Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

  • Journal List
  • Am J Pharm Educ
  • v.81(9); 2017 Nov
  • PMC5738945

Am J Pharm Educ. 2017 Nov; 81(9): 6065.

Abstract

Objective: To address the stages of expertise development, what differentiates a novice from an expert, and how the development and differences impact how we teach our classes or design the curriculum. This paper will also address the downside of expertise and discuss the importance of teaching expertise relative to domain expertise.

Summary: Experts develop through years of experience and by progressing from novice, advance beginner, proficient, competent, and finally expert. These stages are contingent on progressive problem solving, which means individuals must engage in increasingly complex problems, strategically aligned with the learner's stage of development. Thus, several characteristics differentiate experts from novices. Experts know more, their knowledge is better organized and integrated, they have better strategies for accessing knowledge and using it, and they are self-regulated and have different motivations.

Keywords: expert, expertise, novice, instructional design, deliberative practice

INTRODUCTION

Your child wants to play baseball. As a parent, you are weighing options on how to get your child to the goal of becoming a professional baseball player. Option 1 is to go to a local major sports university and have the pitchers throw 90 mph fastballs at your child to improve his or her hitting or hit line drives until he or she adapts or fails at fielding. Option 2, do the same thing but start with little leaguers who are a few years older than your child. Option 3, start with T-ball. Remove the barrier of pitching error. Make sure the basics of hitting and running the bases are down – that first base is down the right field line, not the left field line. You help them learn to catch with two hands and build confidence and skill to prevent from being hit in the face with a pop-up. Once they manage T-ball, progress to little league, you let them go to camps and then advance them to higher leagues as their skillset and competency in baseball develops. Most people would say option 3 makes the most sense. We intuitively understand expertise requires experience and the importance of strategically increasing the challenge of the task. The sink or swim mentality does not work. However, do we do this when teaching? What differentiates a novice from an expert and how does that impact how we teach our classes or design the curriculum?

First, we can accomplish skills with some degree of automaticity, but it does not mean we are experts in that skill. We can acquire most everyday skills at an acceptable level of performance over a short period (eg, driving), maybe as little as 50 hours of practice.1-3 After this time, we can maintain an acceptable level of performance with a minimal amount of cognitive effort. Expertise, however, is more than just performing at an acceptable level semi-automatically.

Experts have built substantial knowledge bases that affect what they notice, and how they organize, represent and interpret information. These adaptations lead to better problem solving and performance. Researchers have investigated the differences between experts and novices in a variety of fields and from this research, we have learned that expertise is more than an accumulation of knowledge or experience. We also learned experience alone is insufficient to guarantee the development of expertise. 1,4,5 The idea that 10,000 hours of practice is needed to develop expertise is a fallacy;6 it may require much more practice and a specific type of practice. The purpose of this review is to discuss differences in experts and novices and based on that development process, identify areas of how instruction, course and curriculum development can be used to educate future health professionals effectively. In addition, we will discuss how being an expert could be a hindrance when training novices and how to overcome the expert-novice divide.

How does expertise develop?

We develop expertise through years of experience, but years of experience do not guarantee an individual will become an expert. Only a small fraction of people become experts; the rest remain as “experienced non-experts.”1 Ullén and colleagues recently found that expertise might have a genetic component which may explain why only a few people become experts in their domain.7 Experts excel mainly in their domains, and there is little evidence that a person highly skilled in one area (eg, medicinal chemistry) can transfer or teach the skill to another (eg, pharmacotherapy).7

The development of expertise occurs over several stages.8 As a rule, a learner cannot directly move from novice to expert; he or she must progress through each stage and may demonstrate characteristics of two stages simultaneously. For instructors, it may be helpful to stage learners to optimize or personalize instruction. There is no clear way to stage a learner, but there are key features and behaviors which may assist in the staging process. Within graduate medical education, the Dreyfus model has been adopted, and we can apply this model to pharmacy education.9,10 The stages in the Dreyfus model include novice, advanced beginner, competent, proficient, and expert.10

In the novice stage (Table 1), pharmacy students are learning the foundational science related to pharmacotherapy (eg, pharmacology, physiology, medicinal chemistry) and the basics of pharmacotherapy. They learn the patient care process including patient history, medication list/history, immunization schedules, and clinical note writing. They will spend their time memorizing facts and are only capable and responsible for following the rules or clearly defined processes.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Novice Stage and Related Instructional Strategies10,41

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

In the advanced beginner stage (Table 2), the learner begins to see aspects of everyday situations. These things can only be learned through experience and the heuristics (rules of thumb) emerge from these experiences to guide the learner. This stage can start because of introductory pharmacy practice experiences (IPPE) but most likely occurs in early advanced pharmacy practice experiences (APPE). Within this stage, essential information is more automatic, less effortful to retrieve but learners still silo information and need assistance making meaningful connections.

Table 2.

Characteristics of the Advanced Beginner Stage and Related Instructional Strategies10,41

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

In the competent stage (Table 3), the learner has increased autonomy and ability to develop patient-specific care plans. When the learner develops a patient care plan, there are risks involved but are attenuated by the preceptor oversight. Because the learner has planned the care, the consequences are foreseeable and present a learning opportunity. This stage may occur in latter APPEs, but most likely this skill is honed during residency training or the first few years of practice. During this stage, the learner starts to develop a routine and is more deliberate in planning for long-term success.

Table 3.

Characteristics of the Competent Stage and Related Instructional Strategies10,41

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

In the proficient stage (Table 4), specialty residents or pharmacists early in practice struggle with developing routines that can streamline patient care. They are managing multiple distractions (e.g., new patients, training learners) but incorporating the intellectual and emotional stimuli into a learning experience. They start to rely more on intuition, display more confidence and accountability. They quickly filter information as pertinent and non-pertinent.

Table 4.

Characteristics of the Proficient Stage and Related Instructional Strategies10,41

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

In the expert stage (Table 5), a mid-career pharmacist has learned to recognize patterns of clues and makes decisions quickly using “intuition.” The pharmacists are attuned to patient nuances and proceed with caution when the unexpected occurs.

Table 5.

Characteristics of the Expert Stage and Related Instructional Strategies10,41

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

How does expertise develop?

The stages of development are contingent on progressive problem solving, which means individuals must engage in increasingly complex problems, strategically aligned with the learner's stage of development. Because progressive problem solving is critical, Ericsson and colleagues proposed the concept of deliberate practice.11 In deliberate practice, problems need to be challenging and relevant to the current situation while also being attainable at each level. Progressive problem-solving seems intuitive to most, however, in practice, we might overlook some of these steps or assume learners are at a different stage. In the classroom paradigm, we may start with cases on a single, uncomplicated disease state. As the learner masters the simple problem, complexities such as comorbidities, social disparities or special populations are added to make the problems challenging, but continually attainable. We develop expertise because of the quality of the design and execution of the learning experience, not just the experience itself.

The theories of deliberate practice suggest that it is more than the number of hours of practice but also the quality of practice that supports the development of expertise.12 Maintaining conscious effort helps to deliberately refine the cognitive skills required to exceed a current level of performance. It should be noted by educators, that this conscious effort will cause discomfort and require work on the part of the learner.1

How do Experts and Novices Differ and how does this impact instruction?

Several characteristics differentiate experts from novices: experts know more, their knowledge is better organized and integrated, they have better strategies for accessing knowledge and using it, and they are self-regulated and have different motivations.4 We will address each of these differences, especially regarding problem-solving, and how these differences can impact instruction, course or curriculum development.

Knowledge Capacity, Patterns, and Organization

Experts are better problem solvers because of the large amount of domain knowledge and organization of information that reflects a deep understanding of the subject matter.13 An expert’s brain organizes their knowledge around core components that guide thinking. This pattern differs from novices who organize knowledge as a list of facts, formulas, or heuristics. Thus, novices approach a problem by slowly searching for a correct formula or heuristic which can slow down the problem-solving process and lead to errors or omissions. In addition, novice heuristics may lead to bias and inappropriate decision making. In one study, medical students were more prone to anchoring to one diagnosis and failing to modify the diagnosis when additional information arose (anchoring bias) and accepted a diagnosis before it had been fully vetted (premature closure).14 When designing instruction, it is important that learners grow their knowledge base while also learning how to organize information to see relationships and linkages to the material within a course and across a curriculum. These ties can be made possible through cases encompassing multiple courses,15 developing concept maps,16 experiential education17 or potentially capstone experiences (Table 6).18

Table 6.

Summary of Experts/Novice Differences and Recommendations on How to Address These Differences in Learning Environments

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

As expertise develops, knowledge transitions from being a collection of isolated facts to a heavily integrated network of information and decision-making process, conditional on a set of circumstances. The conditional knowledge allows experts to retrieve the information they need under the current conditions selectively and includes the contexts in which the information is useful. Novices see knowledge as context dependent and do not easily make connections to other information previously learned (eg, I learned this in this class, it does not apply to other classes or real life). With experience, knowledge becomes decontextualized, but its conditions for use are still evolving. For these reasons, approaching content or problems from various perspectives can help decontextualize the process. Instructors should focus on repeated opportunities for retrieval of information, manipulating testing format (short answer vs. multiple choice vs. generative), spacing testing to optimize retention, providing various contexts to make connections (pharmacology vs. pharmacotherapy), and providing specific feedback including where students can get more help.19

Experts can retrieve important aspects of their knowledge with little attentional effort – that is, their information has a high retrieval strength. With novices, retrieving information places heavy cognitive demands on their attention. Over time and with repeated retrieval of information, the cognitive effort of remembering facts and lists is replaced by making connections and using the information to solve problems. If a student is trying to learn pharmacokinetics and they are not competent in manipulating logarithmic equations, then they will be giving attention to the equation manipulation instead of learning the principles of pharmacokinetics. Therefore, instructors need to scaffold problems to reduce excessively high strains on cognitive load (see article by Sanders and Welk20 for scaffolding tips).

This ease of retrieving information does not mean that experts solve problems faster than novices. An expert can take longer to solve a problem when attempting to understand the problem deeply. Experts may be faster at routine problems, problems that might be onerous for a novice but have become second nature for an expert.21 For routine tasks, experts complete tasks anywhere from 1.3 to multiple times faster.22-24 In one study, experts were 17 times faster than novices in tying laparoscopic knots while using fewer movements.23 From an instructional design standpoint, instructors need to allow sufficient time for novices to solve problems. The allocation of time may be difficult as experts may anchor their judgments on how long it takes themselves to address the problem. The problem-solving process also may frustrate instructors since they tend to take deeper approaches to understanding the problem where novices may take a more superficial approach.

During the problem-solving process, expert pattern recognition differs from novices. An expert detects and identifies problem patterns more readily; they notice features and meaningful connections with the information that are not noticed by novices. For example, an expert clinician may see within a conversation with a heart failure patient a statement about how they sleep on three pillows instead of two. To a novice, this may not be relevant because they are focused on more textbook features or physical assessment but for a clinician, this is an important piece of information that impacts the clinical decision, especially if the information is consistent with other signs or symptoms. An instructor can facilitate this process by having students find themes within the problem or list possibilities that could link to a certain aspect of the problem.

Experts solve problems by forming mental representations of the problem, which are used to infer relationships that define the situation and its constraints. Defining the situation and its constraints leads to a clear picture of the problem and identification of a clear solution. From an instructional standpoint, have learners sketch out the process they will use to identify the problem and have them recognize important and relevant information and less important but still pertinent information related to the problem. It also is important to determine constraints on the problem – what is likely versus least likely. Finally, problems an expert view as easy, may be quite challenging for the novice. This disconnect resolves by using more advanced trainees (upperclassmen, residents) to help vet problems and ensure the complexity is appropriate.

Ultimately, experts want to understand deeply what the problem means or identify what the root of the issue is rather than just plug-in numbers (or facts) in a formula to get an answer. By understanding the nuances of the problem and corresponding constraints, experts can then explain why they used the tactics they did to come to a solution. Novices just want to solve the smaller components of a larger problem which they consider in individual silos and address each element at a superficial level, ultimately treating their knowledge like a list of inflexible and unrelated artifacts. For example, imagine a student-pharmacist approached by a patient presenting with a prescription for benzonatate to treat a cough. The novice may focus on verifying that the order is correct and begin to start thinking about how to best counsel the patient on this medication – the superficial approach. An experienced preceptor would take a more holistic approach and consider the entire patient when verifying the prescription. This process would allow the preceptor to identify if one of the patient’s other medications, like an ACE inhibitor, is causing the cough and ultimately determine that the benzonatate is not needed. The siloed approach used by novices to solve problems superficially without considering the larger picture can frustrate the instructor, therefore, patience is required to coach students through the process. Instructors can remediate this by highlighting the importance of deeply understanding the problem and modeling that behavior explicitly.

Motivation and Self-Regulation

A necessary part of the development of expertise is motivation and self-regulatory processes.25 The expert's motivation focuses on mastery which is associated with persistence toward a goal. Experts continually focus their effort toward improving their knowledge base and skillset to achieve and maintain their expertise. They must constantly practice, be open to challenging new experiences and engage in self- reflective processes.

Due to a high level of understanding of how they know what they know and what they do not know, experts have greater metacognitive awareness than novices. This awareness allows experts to be sensitive to task demands (eg, time, effort, resources needed) and more strategic and flexible in their planning and actions.26 Experts also know when they need to check for errors, the reasons why they fail to comprehend a situation and how they need to redirect their efforts to attain better outcomes. This self-regulation makes experts better learners. For example, experts can predict accurately which problems were challenging and which were easy.27 This awareness enables them to monitor how they should allocate time for solving problems correctly.

This metacognitive ability allows experts to monitor their thinking and problem solving. This helps experts ask more questions especially for difficult concepts.28 Novice learners, on the other hand, ask more questions on the easier material or superficial aspects of the problem. Novice learners are not as likely to monitor their learning and do not have a good idea about whether they have comprehended and mastered the information presented. This phenomenon relates to the Dunning-Kruger effect, a cognitive bias in which relatively unskilled individuals mistakenly assess their ability to be much higher than it is.29, 30 Novices are unlikely to use self-tests and self-question as a source of feedback to correct misinformation or misconceptions or change learning strategies to more efficiently or effectively learn.31 These differences point to a meaningful distinction between experts and novices about learning expert notice when they are not learning and thus are highly likely to seek a strategic remedy when faced with learning difficulties. The solution is to help students develop metacognitive skills so they can become independent monitors of their work.32,33

Is there a downside to Expertise?

Think back to our baseball example presented earlier. If you had to teach a child how to throw a baseball, which has become an automatic process for you, could you accurately articulate the process involved in throwing the ball into a list of directions? You are likely right now picturing someone slowly throwing a ball and trying to identify the smaller movements involved in the process so you can translate those movements into words. Unless you previously coached little league, you probably have not already thought intensively about the nuances involved in throwing a ball. The point being, an expert in a domain, is not inherently proficient in teaching domain specific information to novices. To become an effective educator can take practice, time and dedication. This efficiency experts developed can make identifying the exact process that they go through difficult to articulate since the thinking process itself has become intuitive.

Finally, experts are at risk for metacognitive biases in areas that they once “knew.” Individuals understand their knowledge degrades over time but may be prone to misjudging the rate of this decline.34 By misestimating the rate of decline, individuals rate themselves highly in their domain knowledge (a metacognitive miscalibration). Only after noticing they have knowledge gaps or a domain specific deficiency can the judgments become better calibrated. What is occurring is a misattribution of “peak” knowledge (what I once knew) for current knowledge (what I know now).34 This may impact instructors who are teaching slightly outside their domain expertise or who no longer practice pharmacy. For this reason, experts are always learning and practicing to reduce this metacognitive error – without the practice, they may end up in an arrested development stage.42

Instructional approaches and the Expertise Reversal Effect

Within health professional education we tend to use the clinical or scientific experts to teach within their respective area. To date, there is no correlation, or a slight negative correlation, between research productivity (as a measure of expertise) and measures of teaching effectiveness.35,36 Expertise can hurt student learning because experts forget what is easy and what was difficult to learn. For experts, it all seems natural.37 If experts do not have the pedagogical knowledge, they may be more prone to rely on textbooks on how to teach their students. The book authors do not know anything about their classroom, the students, students’ prior knowledge or other factors that impact instruction or student development.

Individuals who have developed an expertise in teaching know the difficulties that students are likely to encounter when learning. They have a reasonable idea of the level of existing knowledge (or can assess current knowledge) so they can make new information more meaningful. Expert teachers formatively assess a student’s progress to the goal, not just summative at the end. Finally, expert instructors have integrated the content knowledge (domain specific expertise) with the pedagogical knowledge (teaching expertise) that underlies effective teaching. Ultimately if a content expert wants to be an excellent educator, he or she will need to dedicate time and energy to improve instruction continually and work toward becoming an expert teacher.

When novices are learning, especially for very new material, there is a significant burden placed on working memory. This load is reduced after repeated retrieval practice because the information is organized and stored in long-term memory resulting in information processing that is more efficient. There are instructional techniques that facilitate this organization and automation by reducing working memory. However, evidence suggests the effectiveness of these techniques depends on the learner expertise. Instructional methods that are useful for inexperienced learners can lose their effectiveness or have negative consequences when used with more experienced learners. This phenomenon is the expertise reversal effect.38

One of the reasons for this effect is cognitive load. Within the cognitive load framework, there is the germane load (load devoted to processing information, constructing and automating schemas), intrinsic load (load imposed by learning the task) and extraneous load (load imposed by the presentation of information to learners). The concept of the extraneous load is not only related to the capacity or duration of working memory, but also to the amount of cognitive processing. An instructor can impose an extraneous load on the learner if it requires irrelevant cognitive activities, which do not result in learning.39,40 Figure 1 summarizes some of the instructional techniques that may be more helpful for novices and learners with higher baseline knowledge.

Which of the following outcomes is associated with being an expert at something?

Instructional Strategies Based on Level of Expertise38,43,44

SUMMARY

Table 6 summarizes differences between experts and novices and how these differences inform instruction. Expertise develops through deliberate practice. The curriculum should provide an opportunity for students to learn the necessary background knowledge, make meaningful connections to previously learned content and reinforce that information over time. This practice requires intentional scaffolding (lesson, course, and curriculum) and collaboration between course instructors to provide progressively challenging and integrated problems that are appropriate for the learner.

Courses should support students, so they are encouraged to retrieve information actively repeatedly. This retrieval should happen in both structured classroom environments and independently outside of classroom settings. For any class activities, instructors need to provide adequate time for students to complete the assignment. This time-allocation may seem simple, but frequently experts underestimate the time required to solve more common problems. Students may be at different levels, so you need to have the flexibility to match the student ability level with the activity presented.

Finally, students require metacognitive development. There should be a focus on being explicit about the thought processes, especially when it comes to clinical decision making and applying knowledge to increasingly complex situations. Provide precise and actionable feedback, so the learner has the opportunity and support to increase their self-awareness about ability level and confidence.

REFERENCES

1. Ericsson KA. The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]

2. Anderson JR. Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychol Rev.1982;89(4):369–406. [Google Scholar]

3. Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychol Rev.1977;84(2):127–190. [Google Scholar]

4. Chi MTH, Glaser R, Farr MJ. The Nature of Expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]

5. Swanson HL, O'Connor JE, Cooney JB. An information processing analysis of expert and novice teachers' problem solving. Am Educ Res J.1990;27(3):533–556. [Google Scholar]

6. Macnamara BN, Hambrick DZ, Oswald FL. Deliberate practice and performance in music, games, sports, education, and professions: a meta-analysis. Psychol Sci.2014;25(8):1608–1618. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Ullén F, Hambrick DZ, Mosing MA. Rethinking expertise: a multifactorial gene-environment interaction model of expert performance. Psychol Bull.2016;142(4):427–446. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Dreyfus HL, Dreyfus SE. Expertise in real world contexts. Org Studies.2005;26(5):779–792. [Google Scholar]

9. Batalden P, Leach D, Swing S, Dreyfus H, Dreyfus S. General competencies and accreditation in graduate medical education. Health Aff (Millwood).2002;21(5):103–111. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Carraccio CL, Benson BJ, Nixon LJ, Derstine PL. From the educational bench to the clinical bedside: translating the Dreyfus developmental model to the learning of clinical skills. Acad Med.2008;83(8):761–767. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Römer C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev.1993;100(3):363–406. [Google Scholar]

12. Kuhlmann DO, Ardichvili A. Becoming an expert: developing expertise in an applied discipline. Eur J Train Develop.2015;39(4):262–276. [Google Scholar]

13. Johnson PE, Durán AS, Hassebrock F, et al. Expertise and error in diagnostic reasoning. Cogn Sci.1981;5(3):235–283. [Google Scholar]

14. Rylander M, Guerrasio J. Heuristic errors in clinical reasoning. Clin Teach.2016;13(4):287–290. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Kolluru S, Roesch DM, de la Fuente AA. A multi-instructor, team-based, active-learning exercise to integrate basic and clinical sciences content. Am J Pharm Educ.2012;76(2):Article 33. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Daley BJ, Torre DM. Concept maps in medical education: an analytical literature review. Med Educ.2010;44(5):440–448. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. DAa Kolb. Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.; 2015. [Google Scholar]

18. Conway JM, Ahmed GF. A pharmacotherapy capstone course to advance pharmacy students' clinical documentation skills. Am J Pharm Educ.2012;76(7):Article 134. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Larsen D, Butler A. Test-enhanced learning. In: Walsh K, editor. Oxford Textbook of Medical Education. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]

20. Sanders D, Welk DS. Strategies to scaffold student learning: applying Vygotsky's Zone of Proximal Development. Nurse Educ.2005;30(5):203–207. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Schmidt HG, Norman GR, Boshuizen HP. A cognitive perspective on medical expertise: theory and implication. Acad Med.1990;65(10):611–621. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Judkins TN, Oleynikov D, Stergiou N. Objective evaluation of expert and novice performance during robotic surgical training tasks. Surg Endosc.2009;23(3):590–597. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Haji FA, Khan R, Regehr G, Drake J, de Ribaupierre S, Dubrowski A. Measuring cognitive load during simulation-based psychomotor skills training: sensitivity of secondary-task performance and subjective ratings. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract.2015;20(5):1237–1253. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Jaarsma T, Boshuizen HPA, Jarodzka H, Nap M, Verboon P, van Merriënboer JJG. Tracks to a medical diagnosis: expertise differences in visual problem solving. Appl Cogn Psychol.2016;30(3):314–322. [Google Scholar]

25. King G, Currie M, Bartlett DJ, Strachan D, Tucker MA, Willoughby C. The development of expertise in paediatric rehabilitation therapists: the roles of motivation, openness to experience, and types of caseload experience. Aust Occup Ther J. 2008;55(2):108–122. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Weinstein CE, Stone GVM. Broadening our conception of general education: the self-regulated learner. New Dir Comm College.1993;1993(81):31–39. [Google Scholar]

27. Fakcharoenphol W, Morphew JW, Mestre JP. Judgments of physics problem difficulty among experts and novices. Physical Rev Spec Topic Phys Educ Res.2015;11(2) [Google Scholar]

28. Miyake N, Norman DA. To ask a question, one must know enough to know what is not known. J Verb Learn Verb Behav.1979;18(3):357–364. [Google Scholar]

29. Kruger J, Dunning D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J Personal Soc Psychol.1999;77(6):1121–1134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Serra MJ, DeMarree KG. Unskilled and unaware in the classroom: college students’ desired grades predict their biased grade predictions. Mem Cognit.2016;44(7):1127–1137. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Stein BS, Bransford JD, Franks JJ, Vye NJ, Perfetto GA. Differences in judgments of learning difficulty. J Exp Psychol Gen.1982;111(4):406–413. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Medina MS, Castleberry AN, Persky AM. Strategies to improving learner metacognition in health professional education. Am J Pharm Educ.2017;81(4):Article 78. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Fisher M, Keil FC. The curse of expertise: when more knowledge leads to miscalibrated explanatory insight. Cogn Sci.2016;40(5):1251–1269. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Carter RE. Faculty scholarship has a profound positive association with student evaluations of teaching – except when it doesn't. J Market Educ.2016;38(1):18. [Google Scholar]

36. Feldman KA. Research productivity and scholarly accomplishment of college teachers as related to their instructional effectiveness: a review and exploration. Res Higher Educ.1987;26(3):227–298. [Google Scholar]

37. Carraccio CL, Benson BJ, Nixon LJ, Derstine PL. From the educational bench to the clinical bedside: translating the Dreyfus developmental model to the learning of clinical skills. Acad Med.2008;83(8):761–767. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

38. Kalyuga S, Ayres P, Chandler P, Sweller J. The expertise reversal effect. Educ Psychol.2003;38(1):23–31. [Google Scholar]

39. van Merrienboer JJG, Sweller J. Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educ Psychol Rev.2005;17(2):147–177. [Google Scholar]

40. Kalyuga S, Singh A-M. Rethinking the boundaries of cognitive load theory in complex learning. Educ Psychol Rev.2016;28(4):831–852. [Google Scholar]

41. Peña A. The Dreyfus model of clinical problem-solving skills acquisition: a critical perspective. Med Educ Online.2010;15:1–11. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: a general overview. Acad Emerg Med.2008;15(11):988–994. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Rey GD, Buchwald F. The expertise reversal effect: cognitive load and motivational explanations. J Exp Psychol Appl.2011;17(1):33–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

44. Schnotz W. Reanalyzing the expertise reversal effect. Instruct Sci.2010;38(3):315–323. [Google Scholar]


Articles from American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education are provided here courtesy of American Association of Colleges of Pharmacy