The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

This chapter provides an overview of diagnosis in health care, including the committee's conceptual model of the diagnostic process and a review of clinical reasoning. Diagnosis has important implications for patient care, research, and policy. Diagnosis has been described as both a process and a classification scheme, or a “pre-existing set of categories agreed upon by the medical profession to designate a specific condition” (Jutel, 2009).1 When a diagnosis is accurate and made in a timely manner, a patient has the best opportunity for a positive health outcome because clinical decision making will be tailored to a correct understanding of the patient's health problem (Holmboe and Durning, 2014). In addition, public policy decisions are often influenced by diagnostic information, such as setting payment policies, resource allocation decisions, and research priorities (Jutel, 2009; Rosenberg, 2002; WHO, 2012).

The chapter describes important considerations in the diagnostic process, such as the roles of diagnostic uncertainty and time. It also highlights the mounting complexity of health care, due to the ever-increasing options for diagnostic testing2 and treatment, the rapidly rising levels of biomedical and clinical evidence to inform clinical practice, and the frequent comorbidities among patients due to the aging of the population (IOM, 2008, 2013b). The rising complexity of health care and the sheer volume of advances, coupled with clinician time constraints and cognitive limitations, have outstripped human capacity to apply this new knowledge. To help manage this complexity, the chapter concludes with a discussion of the role of clinical practice guidelines in informing decision making in the diagnostic process.

OVERVIEW OF THE DIAGNOSTIC PROCESS

To help frame and organize its work, the committee developed a conceptual model to illustrate the diagnostic process (see Figure 2-1). The committee concluded that the diagnostic process is a complex, patient-centered, collaborative activity that involves information gathering and clinical reasoning with the goal of determining a patient's health problem. This process occurs over time, within the context of a larger health care work system that influences the diagnostic process (see Box 2-1). The committee's depiction of the diagnostic process draws on an adaptation of a decision-making model that describes the cyclical process of information gathering, information integration and interpretation, and forming a working diagnosis (Parasuraman et al., 2000; Sarter, 2014).

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

FIGURE 2-1

The committee's conceptualization of the diagnostic process.

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

The diagnostic process proceeds as follows: First, a patient experiences a health problem. The patient is likely the first person to consider his or her symptoms and may choose at this point to engage with the health care system. Once a patient seeks health care, there is an iterative process of information gathering, information integration and interpretation, and determining a working diagnosis. Performing a clinical history and interview, conducting a physical exam, performing diagnostic testing, and referring or consulting with other clinicians are all ways of accumulating information that may be relevant to understanding a patient's health problem. The information-gathering approaches can be employed at different times, and diagnostic information can be obtained in different orders. The continuous process of information gathering, integration, and interpretation involves hypothesis generation and updating prior probabilities as more information is learned. Communication among health care professionals, the patient, and the patient's family members is critical in this cycle of information gathering, integration, and interpretation.

The working diagnosis may be either a list of potential diagnoses (a differential diagnosis) or a single potential diagnosis. Typically, clinicians will consider more than one diagnostic hypothesis or possibility as an explanation of the patient's symptoms and will refine this list as further information is obtained in the diagnostic process. The working diagnosis should be shared with the patient, including an explanation of the degree of uncertainty associated with a working diagnosis. Each time there is a revision to the working diagnosis, this information should be communicated to the patient. As the diagnostic process proceeds, a fairly broad list of potential diagnoses may be narrowed into fewer potential options, a process referred to as diagnostic modification and refinement (Kassirer et al., 2010). As the list becomes narrowed to one or two possibilities, diagnostic refinement of the working diagnosis becomes diagnostic verification, in which the lead diagnosis is checked for its adequacy in explaining the signs and symptoms, its coherency with the patient's context (physiology, risk factors), and whether a single diagnosis is appropriate. When considering invasive or risky diagnostic testing or treatment options, the diagnostic verification step is particularly important so that a patient is not exposed to these risks without a reasonable chance that the testing or treatment options will be informative and will likely improve patient outcomes.

Throughout the diagnostic process, there is an ongoing assessment of whether sufficient information has been collected. If the diagnostic team members are not satisfied that the necessary information has been collected to explain the patient's health problem or that the information available is not consistent with a diagnosis, then the process of information gathering, information integration and interpretation, and developing a working diagnosis continues. When the diagnostic team members judge that they have arrived at an accurate and timely explanation of the patient's health problem, they communicate that explanation to the patient as the diagnosis.

It is important to note that clinicians do not need to obtain diagnostic certainty prior to initiating treatment; the goal of information gathering in the diagnostic process is to reduce diagnostic uncertainty enough to make optimal decisions for subsequent care (Kassirer, 1989; see section on diagnostic uncertainty). In addition, the provision of treatment can also inform and refine a working diagnosis, which is indicated by the feedback loop from treatment into the information-gathering step of the diagnostic process. This also illustrates the need for clinicians to diagnose health problems that may arise during treatment.

The committee identified four types of information-gathering activities in the diagnostic process: taking a clinical history and interview; performing a physical exam; obtaining diagnostic testing; and sending a patient for referrals or consultations. The diagnostic process is intended to be broadly applicable, including the provision of mental health care. These information-gathering processes are discussed in further detail below.

Clinical History and Interview

Acquiring a clinical history and interviewing a patient provides important information for determining a diagnosis and also establishes a solid foundation for the relationship between a clinician and the patient. A common maxim in medicine attributed to William Osler is: “Just listen to your patient, he is telling you the diagnosis” (Gandhi, 2000, p. 1087). An appointment begins with an interview of the patient, when a clinician compiles a patient's medical history or verifies that the details of the patient's history already contained in the patient's medical record are accurate. A patient's clinical history includes documentation of the current concern, past medical history, family history, social history, and other relevant information, such as current medications (prescription and over-the-counter) and dietary supplements.

The process of acquiring a clinical history and interviewing a patient requires effective communication, active listening skills, and tailoring communication to the patient based on the patient's needs, values, and preferences. The National Institute on Aging, in guidance for conducting a clinical history and interview, suggests that clinicians should avoid interrupting, demonstrate empathy, and establish a rapport with patients (NIA, 2008). Clinicians need to know when to ask more detailed questions and how to create a safe environment for patients to share sensitive information about their health and symptoms. Obtaining a history can be challenging in some cases: For example, in working with older adults with memory loss, with children, or with individuals whose health problems limit communication or reliable self-reporting. In these cases it may be necessary to include family members or caregivers in the history-taking process. The time pressures often involved in clinical appointments also contribute to challenges in the clinical history and interview. Limited time for clinical visits, partially attributed to payment policies (see Chapter 7), may lead to an incomplete picture of a patient's relevant history and current signs and symptoms.

There are growing concerns that traditional “bedside evaluation” skills (history, interview, and physical exam) have received less attention due the large growth in diagnostic testing in medicine. Verghese and colleagues noted that these methods were once the primary tools for diagnosis and clinical evaluation, but “the recent explosion of imaging and laboratory testing has inverted the diagnostic paradigm. [Clinicians] often bypass the bedside evaluation for immediate testing” (Verghese et al., 2011, p. 550). The interview has been called a clinician's most versatile diagnostic and therapeutic tool, and the clinical history provides direction for subsequent information-gathering activities in the diagnostic process (Lichstein, 1990). An accurate history facilitates a more productive and efficient physical exam and the appropriate utilization of diagnostic testing (Lichstein, 1990). Indeed, Kassirer concluded: “Diagnosis remains fundamentally dependent on a personal interaction of a [clinician] with a patient, the sufficiency of communication between them, the accuracy of the patient's history and physical examination, and the cognitive energy necessary to synthesize a vast array of information” (Kassirer, 2014, p. 12).

Physical Exam

The physical exam is a hands-on observational examination of the patient. First, a clinician observes a patient's demeanor, complexion, posture, level of distress, and other signs that may contribute to an understanding of the health problem (Davies and Rees, 2010). If the clinician has seen the patient before, these observations can be weighed against previous interactions with the patient. A physical exam may include an analysis of many parts of the body, not just those suspected to be involved in the patient's current complaint. A careful physical exam can help a clinician refine the next steps in the diagnostic process, can prevent unnecessary diagnostic testing, and can aid in building trust with the patient (Verghese, 2011). There is no universally agreed upon physical examination checklist; myriad versions exist online and in textbooks.

Due to the growing emphasis on diagnostic testing, there are concerns that physical exam skills have been underemphasized in current health care professional education and training (Kassirer, 2014; Kugler and Verghese, 2010). For example, Kugler and Verghese have asserted that there is a high degree in variability in the way that trainees elicit physical signs and that residency programs have not done enough to evaluate and improve physical exam techniques. Physicians at Stanford have developed the “Stanford 25,” a list of physical diagnostic maneuvers that are very technique-dependent (Verghese and Horwitz, 2009). Educators observe students and residents performing these 25 maneuvers to ensure that trainees are able to elicit the physical signs reliably (Stanford Medicine 25 Team, 2015).

Diagnostic Testing

Over the past 100 years, diagnostic testing has become a critical feature of standard medical practice (Berger, 1999; European Society of Radiology, 2010). Diagnostic testing may occur in successive rounds of information gathering, integration, and interpretation, as each round of information refines the working diagnosis. In many cases, diagnostic testing can identify a condition before it is clinically apparent; for example, coronary artery disease can be identified by an imaging study indicating the presence of coronary artery blockage even in the absence of symptoms.

The primary emphasis of this section focuses on laboratory medicine, anatomic pathology, and medical imaging (see Box 2-2). However, there are many important forms of diagnostic testing that extend beyond these fields, and the committee's conceptual model is intended to be broadly applicable. Aditional forms of diagnostic testing include, for example, screening tools used in making mental health diagnoses (SAMHSA and HRSA, 2015), sleep apnea testing, neurocognitive assessment, and vision and hearing testing.

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

BOX 2-2

Laboratory Medicine, Anatomic Pathology, and Medical Imaging.

Although it was developed specifically for laboratory medicine, the brain-to-brain loop model is useful for describing the general process of diagnostic testing (Lundberg, 1981; Plebani et al., 2011). The model includes nine steps: test selection and ordering, sample collection, patient identification, sample transportation, sample preparation, sample analysis, result reporting, result interpretation, and clinical action (Lundberg, 1981). These steps occur during five phases of diagnostic testing: prepre-analytic, pre-analytic, analytic, post-analytic, and post-post-analytic phases. Errors related to diagnostic testing can occur in any of these five phases, but the analytic phase is the least susceptible to errors (Eichbaum et al., 2012; Epner et al., 2013; Laposata, 2010; Nichols and Rauch, 2013; Stratton, 2011) (see Chapter 3).

The pre-pre-analytic phase, which involves clinician test selection and ordering, has been identified as a key point of vulnerability in the work process due to the large number and variety of available tests, which makes it difficult for nonspecialist clinicians to accurately select the correct test or series of tests (Hickner et al., 2014; Laposata and Dighe, 2007). The pre-analytic phase involves sample collection, patient identification, sample transportation, and sample preparation. During the analytic phase, the specimen is tested, examined, or both. Adequate performance in this phase depends on the correct execution of a chemical analysis or morphological examination (Hollensead et al., 2004), and the contribution to diagnostic errors at this step is small. The post-analytic phase includes the generation of results, reporting, interpretation, and follow-up. Ensuring accurate and timely reporting from the laboratory to the ordering clinician and patient is central to this phase. During the post-post-analytic phase, the ordering clinician, sometimes in consultation with pathologists, incorporates the test results into the patient's clinical context, considers the probability of a particular diagnosis in light of the test results, and considers the harms and benefits of future tests and treatments, given the newly acquired information. Possible factors contributing to failure in this phase include an incorrect interpretation of the test result by the ordering clinician or pathologist and the failure by the ordering clinician to act on the test results: for example, not ordering a follow-up test or not providing treatment consistent with the test results (Hickner et al., 2014; Laposata and Dighe, 2007; Plebani and Lippi, 2011).

The medical imaging work process parallels the work process described for pathology. There is a pre-pre-analytic phase (the selection and ordering of medical imaging), a pre-analytic phase (preparing the patient for imaging), an analytic phase (image acquisition and analysis), a post-analytic phase (the imaging results are interpreted and reported to the ordering clinician or the patient), and a post-post-analytic phase (the integration of results into the patient context and further action). The relevant differences between the medical imaging and pathology processes include the nature of the examination and the methods and technology used to interpret the results.

Laboratory Medicine and Anatomic Pathology

In 2008 a Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) report described pathology as an “essential element of the health care system,” stating that pathology is “integral to many clinical decisions, providing physicians, nurses, and other health care providers with often pivotal information for the prevention, diagnosis, treatment, and management of disease” (CDC, 2008, p. 19). Primary care clinicians order laboratory tests in slightly less than one third of patient visits (CDC, 2010; Hickner et al., 2014), and direct-to-patient testing is becoming increasingly prevalent (CDC, 2008). There are now thousands of molecular diagnostic tests available, and this number is expected to increase as the mechanisms of disease at the molecular level are better understood (CDC, 2008; Johansen Taber et al., 2014) (see Box 2-3).

The task of selecting the appropriate diagnostic testing is challenging for clinicians, in part because of the sheer volume of choices. For example, Hickner and colleagues (2014) found that primary care clinicians report uncertainty in ordering laboratory medicine tests in approximately 15 percent of diagnostic encounters. Choosing the appropriate test requires understanding the patient's history and current signs and symptoms, as well as having a sufficient suspicion or pre-test probability of a disease or condition (see section on probabilistic reasoning) (Pauker and Kassirer, 1975, 1980; Sox, 1986). The likelihood of disease is inherently uncertain in this step; for instance, the clinician's patient population may not reflect epidemiological data, and the patient's history can be incomplete or otherwise complicated. Advances in molecular diagnostic technologies and new diagnostic tests have introduced another layer of complexity. Many clinicians are struggling to keep up with the growing availability of such tests and have uncertainty about the best application of these tests in screening, diagnosis, and treatment (IOM, 2015a; Johansen Taber et al., 2014).

Diagnostic tests have “operating parameters,” including sensitivity and specificity that are particular to the diagnostic test for a specific disorder (see section on probabilistic reasoning). Even if a test is performed correctly, there is a chance for a false positive or false negative result. Test interpretation involves reviewing numerical or qualitative (yes or no) results and combining those results with patient history, symptoms, and pretest disease likelihood. Test interpretation needs to be patient-specific and to consider information learned during the physical exam and the clinical history and interview. Several studies have highlighted test interpretation errors, such as the misinterpretation of a false positive human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) screening test for a low-risk patient as indicative of HIV infection (Gigerenzer, 2013; Kleinman et al., 1998). In addition, test performance may only be characterized in a limited patient population, leading to challenges with generalizability (Whiting et al., 2004).

The laboratories that conduct diagnostic testing are some of the most regulated and inspected areas in health care (see Table 2-1). Some of the relevant entities include The Joint Commission and other accreditors, the federal government, and various other organizations, such as the College of American Pathologists (CAP) and the American Society for Clinical Pathology. There are many ways in which quality is assessed. Examples include proficiency testing of clinical laboratory assays and pathologists (e.g., Pap smear proficiency testing), many of which are regulated under the Clinical Laboratory Improvement Amendments, and inter-laboratory comparison programs (e.g., CAP's Q-Probes, Q-Monitors, and Q-Tracks programs).

Medical Imaging

Medical imaging plays a critical role in establishing the diagnoses for innumerable conditions and it is used routinely in nearly every branch of medicine. The advancement of imaging technologies has improved the ability of clinicians to detect, diagnose, and treat conditions while also allowing patients to avoid more invasive procedures (European Society of Radiology, 2010; Gunderman, 2005). For many conditions (e.g., brain tumors), imaging is the only noninvasive diagnostic method available. The appropriate choice of imaging modality depends on the disease, organ, and specific clinical questions to be addressed. Computed tomography (CT) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) are first-line methods for assessing conditions of the central and peripheral nervous system, while for musculoskeletal and a variety of other conditions, X-ray and ultrasound are often employed first because of their relatively low cost and ready availability, with CT and MRI being reserved as problem-solving modalities. CT procedures are frequently used to assess and diagnose cancer, circulatory system diseases and conditions, inflammatory diseases, and head and internal organ injuries. A majority of MRI procedures are performed on the spine, brain, and musculoskeletal system, although usage for the breast, prostate, abdominal, and pelvic regions is rising (IMV, 2014).

Medical imaging is characterized not just by the increasingly precise anatomic detail it offers but also by an increasing capacity to illuminate biology. For example, magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging has allowed the assessment of metabolism, and a growing number of other MRI sequences are offering information about functional characteristics, such as blood perfusion or water diffusion. In addition, several new tracers for molecular imaging with PET (typically as PET/CT) have recently been approved for clinical use, and more are undergoing clinical trials, while PET/MRI was recently introduced to the clinical setting. Functional and molecular imaging data may be assessed qualitatively, quantitatively, or both. Although other forms of diagnostic testing can identify a wide array of molecular markers, molecular imaging is unique in its capacity to noninvasively show the locations of molecular processes in patients, and it is expected to play a critical role in advancing precision medicine, particularly for cancers, which often demonstrate both intra- and intertumoral biological heterogeneity (Hricak, 2011).

The growing body of medical knowledge, the variety of imaging options available, and the regular increases in the amounts and kinds of data that can be captured with imaging present tremendous challenges for radiologists, as no individual can be expected to achieve competency in all of the imaging modalities. General radiologists continue to be essential in certain clinical settings, but extended training and sub-specialization are often necessary for optimal, clinically relevant image interpretation, as is involvement in multidisciplinary disease management teams. Furthermore, the use of structured reporting templates tailored to specific examinations can help to increase the clarity, thoroughness, and clinical relevance of image interpretation (Schwartz et al., 2011).

Like other forms of diagnostic testing, medical imaging has limitations. Some studies have found that between 20 and 50 percent of all advanced imaging results fail to provide information that improves patient outcome, although these studies do not account for the value of negative imaging results in influencing decisions about patient management (Hendee et al., 2010). Imaging may fail to provide useful information because of modality sensitivity and specificity parameters; for example, the spatial resolution of an MRI may not be high enough to detect very small abnormalities. Inadequate patient education and preparation for an imaging test can also lead to suboptimal imaging quality that results in diagnostic error.

Perceptual or cognitive errors made by radiologists are a source of diagnostic error (Berlin, 2014; Krupinski et al., 2012). In addition, incomplete or incorrect patient information, as well as insufficient sharing of patient information, may lead to the use of an inadequate imaging protocol, an incorrect interpretation of imaging results, or the selection of an inappropriate imaging test by a referring clinician. Referring clinicians often struggle with selecting the appropriate imaging test, in part because of the large number of available imaging options and gaps in the teaching of radiology in medical schools. Although consensus-based guidelines (e.g., the various “appropriateness criteria” published by the American College of Radiology [ACR]) are available to help select imaging tests for many conditions, these guidelines are often not followed. The use of clinical decision support systems at the point of care as well as direct consultations with radiologists have been proposed by the ACR as methods for improving imaging test selection (Allen and Thorwarth, 2014).

There are several mechanisms for ensuring the quality of medical imaging. The Mammography Quality Standards Act (MQSA)—overseen by the Food and Drug Administration—was the first government-mandated accreditation program for any type of medical facility; it was focused on X-ray imaging for breast cancer. MQSA provides a general framework for ensuring national quality standards in facilities that perform screening mammography (IOM, 2005). MQSA requires all personnel at facilities to meet initial qualifications, to demonstrate continued experience, and to complete continuing education. MQSA addresses protocol selection, image acquisition, interpretation and report generation, and the communication of results and recommendations. In addition, it provides facilities with data on diagnostic performance that can be used for benchmarking, self-monitoring, and improvement. MQSA has decreased the variability in mammography performed across the United States and improved the quality of care (Allen and Thorwarth, 2014). However, the ACR noted that MQSA is complex and specified in great detail, which makes it inflexible, leading to administrative burdens and the need for extensive training of staff for implementation (Allen and Thorwarth, 2014). It also focuses on only one medical imaging modality in one disease area; thus, it does not address newer screening technologies (IOM, 2005). In addition, the Medicare Improvements for Patients and Providers Act (MIPPA)3 requires that private outpatient facilities that perform CT, MRI, breast MRI, nuclear medicine, and PET exams be accredited. The requirements include personnel qualifications, image quality, equipment performance, safety standards, and quality assurance and quality control (ACR, 2015a). There are four CMS-designated accreditation organizations for medical imaging: ACR, the Intersocietal Accreditation Commission, The Joint Commission, and RadSite (CMS, 2015a). MIPPA also mandated that, beginning in 2017, ordering clinicians will be required to consult appropriateness criteria to order advanced medical imaging procedures, and the act called for a demonstration project evaluating clinician compliance with appropriateness criteria (Timbie et al., 2014). In addition to these mandated activities, societies such as ACR and the Radiological Society of North America (RSNA) provide quality improvement programs and resources (ACR, 2015b; RSNA, 2015).

Referral and Consultation

Clinicians may refer to or consult with other clinicians (formally or informally) to seek additional expertise about a patient's health problem. The consult may help to confirm or reject the working diagnosis or may provide information on potential treatment options. If a patient's health problem is outside a clinician's area of expertise, he or she can refer the patient to a clinician who holds more suitable expertise. Clinicians can also recommend that the patient seek a second opinion from another clinician to verify their impressions of an uncertain diagnosis or if they believe that this would be helpful to the patient. Many groups raise awareness that patients can obtain a second opinion on their own (AMA, 1996; CMS, 2015c; PAF, 2012). Diagnostic consultations can also be arranged through the use of integrated practice units or diagnostic management teams (Govern, 2013; Porter, 2010; see Chapter 4).

IMPORTANT CONSIDERATIONS IN THE DIAGNOSTIC PROCESS

The committee elaborated on several aspects of the diagnostic process which are discussed below, including

  • diagnostic uncertainty

  • time

  • population trends

  • diverse populations and health disparities

  • mental health

Diagnostic Uncertainty

One of the complexities in the diagnostic process is the inherent uncertainty in diagnosis. As noted in the committee's conceptual model of the diagnostic process, an overarching question throughout the process is whether sufficient information has been collected to make a diagnosis. This does not mean that a diagnosis needs to be absolutely certain in order to initiate treatment. Kassirer concluded that:

Absolute certainty in diagnosis is unattainable, no matter how much information we gather, how many observations we make, or how many tests we perform. A diagnosis is a hypothesis about the nature of a patient's illness, one that is derived from observations by the use of inference. As the inferential process unfolds, our confidence as [clinicians] in a given diagnosis is enhanced by the gathering of data that either favor it or argue against competing hypotheses. Our task is not to attain certainty, but rather to reduce the level of diagnostic uncertainty enough to make optimal therapeutic decisions. (Kassirer, 1989, p. 1489)

Thus, the probability of disease does not have to be equal to one (diagnostic certainty) in order for treatment to be justified (Pauker and Kassirer, 1980). The decision to begin treatment based on a working diagnosis is informed by: (1) the degree of certainty about the diagnosis; (2) the harms and benefits of treatment; and (3) the harms and benefits of further information-gathering activities, including the impact of delaying treatment.

The risks associated with diagnostic testing are important considerations when conducting information-gathering activities in the diagnostic process. While underuse of diagnostic testing has been a long-standing concern, overly aggressive diagnostic strategies have recently been recognized for their risks (Zhi et al., 2013) (see Chapter 3). Overuse of diagnostic testing has been partially attributed to clinicians' fear of missing something important and intolerance of diagnostic uncertainty: “I am far more concerned about doing too little than doing too much. It's the scan, the test, the operation that I should have done that sticks with me—sometimes for years. . . . By contrast, I can't remember anyone I sent for an unnecessary CT scan or operated on for questionable reasons a decade ago” (Gawande, 2015). However, there is growing recognition that overly aggressive diagnostic pursuits are putting patients at greater risk for harm, and they are not improving diagnostic certainty (Kassirer, 1989; Welch, 2015).

When considering diagnostic testing options, the harm from the procedure itself needs to be weighed against the potential information that could be gained. For some patients, the risk of invasive diagnostic testing may be inappropriate due to the risk of mortality or morbidity from the test itself (such as cardiac catheterization or invasive biopsies). In addition, the risk for harm needs to take into account the cascade of diagnostic testing and treatment decisions that could stem from a diagnostic test result. Included in these assessments are the potential for false positives and ambiguous or slightly abnormal test results that lead to further diagnostic testing or unnecessary treatment.

There are some cases in which treatment is initiated even though there is limited certainty in a working diagnosis. For example, an individual who has been exposed to a tick bite or HIV may be treated with prophylactic antibiotics or antivirals, because the risk of treatment may be felt to be smaller than the risk of harm from tick-borne diseases or HIV infection. Clinicians sometimes employ empiric treatment strategies—or the provision of treatment with a very uncertain diagnosis—and use a patient's response to treatment as an information-gathering activity to help arrive at a working diagnosis. However, it is important to note that response rates to treatment can be highly variable, and the failure to respond to treatment does not necessarily reflect that a diagnosis is incorrect. Nor does improvement in the patient's condition necessarily validate that the treatment conferred this benefit and, therefore, that the empirically tested diagnosis was in fact correct. A treatment that is beneficial for some patients might not be beneficial for others with the same condition (Kent and Hayward, 2007), hence the interest in precision medicine, which is hoped to better tailor therapy to maximize efficacy and minimize toxicity (Jameson and Longo, 2015). In addition, there are isolated cases where the morbidity and the mortality of a diagnostic procedure and the likelihood of disease is sufficiently high that significant therapy has been given empirically. Moroff and Pauker (1983) described a decision analysis in which a 90-year-old practicing lawyer with a new 1.5 centimeter lung nodule was deemed to have a sufficiently high risk for mortality from lung biopsy and high likelihood of malignancy that the radiation oncologists felt comfortable treating the patient empirically for suspected lung cancer.

Time

Of major importance in the diagnostic process is the element of time. Most diseases evolve over time, and there can be a delay between the onset of disease and the onset of a patient's symptoms; time can also elapse before a patient's symptoms are recognized as a specific diagnosis (Zwaan and Singh, 2015). Some diagnoses can be determined in a very short time frame, while months may elapse before other diagnoses can be made. This is partially due to the growing recognition of the variability and complexity of disease presentation. Similar symptoms may be related to a number of different diagnoses, and symptoms may evolve in different ways as a disease progresses; for example, a disease affecting multiple organs may initially involve symptoms or signs from a single organ. The thousands of different diseases and health conditions do not present in thousands of unique ways; there are only a finite number of symptoms with which a patient may present. At the outset, it can be very difficult to determine which particular diagnosis is indicated by a particular combination of symptoms, especially if symptoms are nonspecific, such as fatigue. Diseases may also present atypically, with an unusual and unexpected constellation of symptoms (Emmett, 1998).

Adding to the complexity of the time-dependent nature of the diagnostic process are the numerous settings of care in which diagnosis occurs and the potential involvement of multiple settings of care within a single diagnostic process. Henriksen and Brady noted that this process—for patients, their families, and clinicians alike—can often feel like “a disjointed journey across confusing terrain, aided or impeded by different agents, with no destination in sight and few landmarks along the way” (Henriksen and Brady, 2013, p. ii2).

Some diagnoses may be more important to establish immediately than others. These include diagnoses that can lead to significant patient harm if not recognized, diagnosed, and treated early, such as anthrax, aortic dissection, and pulmonary embolism. Sometimes making a timely diagnosis relies on the fast recognition of symptoms outside of the health care setting (e.g., public awareness of stroke symptoms can help improve the speed of receiving medical help and increase the chances of a better recovery) (National Stroke Association, 2015). In these cases, the benefit of treating the disease promptly can greatly exceed the potential harm from unnecessary treatment. Consequently, the threshold for ordering diagnostic testing or for initiating treatment becomes quite low for such health problems (Pauker and Kassirer, 1975, 1980). In other cases, the potential harm from rapidly and unnecessarily treating a diagnosed condition can lead to a more conservative (or higher-threshold) approach in the diagnostic process.

Population trends, such as the aging of the population, are adding significant complexity to the diagnostic process and require clinicians to consider such complicating factors in diagnosis as comorbidity, polypharmacy and attendant medication side effects, as well as disease and medication interactions (IOM, 2008, 2013b). Diagnosis can be especially challenging in older patients because classic presentations of disease are less common in older adults (Jarrett et al., 1995). For example, infections such as pneumonia or urinary tract infections often do not present in older patients with fever, cough, and pain but rather with symptoms such as lethargy, incontinence, loss of appetite, or disruption of cognitive function (Mouton et al., 2001). Acute myocardial infarction (MI) may present with fatigue and confusion rather than with typical symptoms such as chest pain or radiating arm pain (Bayer et al., 1986; Qureshi et al., 2000; Rich, 2006). Sensory limitations in older adults, such as hearing and vision impairments, can also contribute to challenges in making diagnoses (Campbell et al., 1999). Physical illnesses often present with a change in cognitive status in older individuals without dementia (Mouton et al., 2001). In older adults with mild to moderate dementia, such illnesses can manifest with worsening cognition. Older patients who have multiple comorbidities, medications, or cognitive and functional impairments are more likely to have atypical disease presentations, which may increase the risk of experiencing diagnostic errors (Gray-Miceli, 2008).

Diverse Populations and Health Disparities

Communicating with diverse populations can also contribute to the complexity of the diagnostic process. Language, health literacy, and cultural barriers can affect clinician–patient encounters and increase the potential for challenges in the diagnostic process (Flores, 2006; IOM, 2003; The Joint Commission, 2007). There are indications that biases influence diagnosis; one well-known example is the differential referral of patients for cardiac catheterization by race and gender (Schulman et al., 1999). In addition, women are more likely than men to experience a missed diagnosis of heart attack, a situation that has been partly attributed to real and perceived gender biases, but which may also be the result of physiologic differences, as women have a higher likelihood of presenting with atypical symptoms, including abdominal pain, shortness of breath, and congestive heart failure (Pope et al., 2000).

Mental Health

Mental health diagnoses can be particularly challenging. Mental health diagnoses rely on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM); each diagnosis in the DSM includes a set of diagnostic criteria that indicate the type and length of symptoms that need to be present, as well as the symptoms, disorders, and conditions that cannot be present, in order to be considered for a particular diagnosis (APA, 2015). Compared to physical diagnoses, many mental health diagnoses rely on patient reports and observation; there are few biological tests that are used in such diagnoses (Pincus, 2014). A key challenge can be distinguishing physical diagnoses from mental health diagnoses; sometimes physical conditions manifest as psychiatric ones, and vice versa (Croskerry, 2003a; Hope et al., 2014; Pincus, 2014; Reeves et al., 2010). In addition, there are concerns about missing psychiatric diagnoses, as well as overtreatment concerns (Bor, 2015; Meyer and Meyer, 2009; Pincus, 2014). For example, clinician biases toward older adults can contribute to missed diagnoses of depression, because it may be perceived that older adults are likely to be depressed, lethargic, or have little interest in interactions. Patients with mental health–related symptoms may also be more vulnerable to diagnostic errors, a situation that is attributed partly to clinician biases; for example, clinicians may disregard symptoms in patients with previous diagnoses of mental illness or substance abuse and attribute new physical symptoms to a psychological cause (Croskerry, 2003a). Individuals with health problems that are difficult to diagnose or those who have chronic pain may also be more likely to receive psychiatric diagnoses erroneously.

CLINICAL REASONING AND DIAGNOSIS

Accurate, timely, and patient-centered diagnosis relies on proficiency in clinical reasoning, which is often regarded as the clinician's quintessential competency. Clinical reasoning is “the cognitive process that is necessary to evaluate and manage a patient's medical problems” (Barrows, 1980, p. 19). Understanding the clinical reasoning process and the factors that can impact it are important to improving diagnosis, given that clinical reasoning processes contribute to diagnostic errors (Croskerry, 2003a; Graber, 2005). Health care professionals involved in the diagnostic process have an obligation and ethical responsibility to employ clinical reasoning skills: “As an expanding body of scholarship further elucidates the causes of medical error, including the considerable extent to which medical errors, particularly in diagnostics, may be attributable to cognitive sources, insufficient progress in systematically evaluating and implementing suggested strategies for improving critical thinking skills and medical judgment is of mounting concern” (Stark and Fins, 2014, p. 386). Clinical reasoning occurs within clinicians' minds (facilitated or impeded by the work system) and involves judgment under uncertainty, with a consideration of possible diagnoses that might explain symptoms and signs, the harms and benefits of diagnostic testing and treatment for each of those diagnoses, and patient preferences and values.

The current understanding of clinical reasoning is based on the dual process theory, a widely accepted paradigm of decision making. The dual process theory integrates analytical and non-analytical models of decision making (see Box 2-4). Analytical models (slow system 2) involve a conscious, deliberate process guided by critical thinking (Kahneman, 2011). Nonanalytical models (fast system 1) involve unconscious, intuitive, and automatic pattern recognition (Kahneman, 2011).

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

BOX 2-4

Models of Clinical Reasoning.

Fast system 1 (nonanalytical, intuitive) automatic processes require very little working memory capacity. They are often triggered by stimuli or result from overlearned associations or implicitly learned activities.4 Examples of system 1 processes include the ability to recognize human faces (Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006), the diagnosis of Lyme disease from a bull's-eye rash, or decisions based on heuristics (mental shortcuts), intuition, or repeated experiences.

In contrast, slow system 2 (reflective, analytical) processing places a heavy load on working memory and involves hypothetical and counterfactual reasoning (Evans and Stanovich, 2013; Stanovich and Toplak, 2012). System 2 processing requires individuals to generate mental models of what should or should not happen in particular situations, in order to test possible actions or to explore alternative causes of events (Stanovich, 2009). Hypothetical thinking occurs when one reasons about what should occur if some condition held: For example, if this patient has diabetes, then the blood sugar level should exceed 126 mg/dl after an 8-hour fast, or if prescribed a diabetes medication, the sugar level should improve. Counterfactual reasoning occurs when one thinks about what should occur if the situation differed from how it actually is. The deliberate, conscious, and reflective nature of both hypothetical and counterfactual reasoning illustrates the analytical nature of system 2.

Heuristics—mental shortcuts or cognitive strategies that are automatically and unconsciously employed—are particularly important for decision making (Gigerenzer and Goldstein, 1996). Heuristics can facilitate decision making but can also lead to errors, especially when patients present with atypical symptoms (Cosmides and Tooby, 1996; Gigerenzer, 2000; Kahneman, 2011; Klein, 1998; Lipshitz et al., 2001; McDonald, 1996). When a heuristic fails, it is referred to as a cognitive bias. Cognitive biases, or predispositions to think in a way that leads to failures in judgment, can also be caused by affect and motivation (Kahneman, 2011). Prolonged learning in a regular and predictable environment increases the success-fulness of heuristics, whereas uncertain and unpredictable environments are a chief cause of heuristic failure (Kahneman, 2011; Kahneman and Klein, 2009). There are many heuristics and biases that affect clinical reasoning and decision making (see Table 2-2 for medical and nonmedical examples). Additional examples of heuristics and biases that affect decision making and the potential for diagnostic errors are described below (Croskerry, 2003b):

TABLE 2-2

Examples of Heuristics and Biases That Influence Decision Making.

  • The representativeness heuristic answers the question, “how likely is it that this patient has a particular disease?” by assessing how typical the patient's symptoms are for that disease. If the symptoms are highly typical (e.g., fever and nausea after contact with an individual from West Africa with Ebola virus), then it is likely the patient will be diagnosed as having that condition (e.g., Ebola virus infection). The representativeness bias refers to the tendency to make decisions based on a typical case, even when this may lead to an incorrect judgment. The representativeness bias helps to explain why an incorrect diagnosis (e.g., a patient diagnosed as not having Ebola virus infection) is made when presenting symptoms are atypical (e.g., no fever or nausea after contact with a person from West Africa).

  • Base-rate neglect describes the tendency to ignore the prevalence of a disease in determining a diagnosis. For example, a clinician may think the diagnosis is acid reflux because it is a prevalent condition, even though it is actually an MI, which can present with similar symptoms (e.g., chest pain), but is less likely.

  • The overconfidence bias reflects the universal tendency to believe that we know more than we do. This bias encourages individuals to diagnose a disease based on incomplete information; too much faith is placed in one's opinion rather than on carefully gathering evidence. This bias is especially likely to develop if clinicians do not have feedback on their diagnostic performance.

  • Psych-out errors describe the increased susceptibility of people with mental illnesses to clinician biases and heuristics due to their mental health conditions. Patients with mental health issues may have new physical symptoms that are not considered seriously because their clinicians attribute them to their mental health issues. Patients with physical symptoms that mimic mental illnesses (hypoxia, delirium, metabolic abnormalities, central nervous infections, and head injuries) may also be susceptible to these errors.

In addition to cognitive biases, research suggests that fallacies in reasoning, ethical violations, and financial and nonfinancial conflicts of interest can influence medical decision making (Seshia et al., 2014a,b). These factors, collectively referred to as “cognitive biases plus,” have been identified as potentially undermining the evidence that informs clinical decision making (Seshia et al., 2014a,b).

The interaction between fast system 1 and slow system 2 remains controversial. Some hold that these processes are constantly occurring in parallel and that any conflicts are resolved as they arise. Others have argued that system 1 processes generate an individual's default response and that system 2 processes may or may not intervene and override system 1 processing (Evans and Stanovich, 2013; Kahneman, 2011). When system 2 overrides system 1, this can lead to improved decision making, because engaging in analytical reasoning may correct for inaccuracies. It is important to note that slow system 2 processing does not guarantee correct decision making. For instance, clinicians with an inadequate knowledge base may not have the information necessary to make a correct decision. There are some instances when system 1 processing is correct, and the override from system 2 can contribute to incorrect decision making. However, when system 1 overrides system 2 processing, this can also result in irrational decision making.

Intervention by system 2 is likely to occur in novel situations when the task at hand is difficult; when an individual has minimal knowledge or experience (Evans and Stanovich, 2013; Kahneman, 2011); or when an individual deliberately employs strategies to overcome known biases (Croskerry et al., 2013). Monitoring and intervention by system 2 on system 1 is unlikely to catch every failure because it is inefficient and would require sustained vigilance, given that system 1 processing often leads to correct solutions (Kahneman, 2011). Factors that affect working memory can impede the ability of system 2 to monitor and, when necessary, intervene on system 1 processes (Croskerry, 2009b). For example, if clinicians are tired or distracted by elements in the work system, they may fail to recognize when a decison provided by system 1 processing needs to be reconsidered (Croskerry, 2009b).

System 1 and system 2 perform optimally in different types of clinical practice settings. System 1 performs best in highly reliable and predictable environments but falls short in uncertain and irregular settings (Kahneman and Klein, 2009; Stanovich, 2009). System 2 performs best in relaxed and unhurried environments.

Dual Process Theory and Diagnosis

This section applies the dual process theory of clinical reasoning to the diagnostic process (Croskerry, 2009a,b; Norman and Eva, 2010; Pelaccia et al., 2011). Croskerry and colleagues provide a framework for understanding the cognitive activities that occur in clinicians as they iterate through information gathering, information integration and interpretation, and determining a working diagnosis (Croskerry et al., 2013) (see Figure 2-2).

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

FIGURE 2-2

The dual process model of diagnostic decision making. When a patient presents to a clinician, the initial data include symptoms and signs of disease, which can range from single characteristics of disease to illness scripts. If the symptoms and signs (more...)

When patients present, clinicians gather information and compare that information with their knowledge about various diseases. This can include comparing a patient's signs and symptoms with clinicians' mental models of diseases (or information about diseases that is stored in memory as exemplars, prototypes, or illness scripts; see Box 2-4). This initial pattern matching is an instance of fast system 1 processing. If a sufficiently unique match occurs, then a diagnosis may be made without involvement of slow system 2.

However, some symptoms or signs may not be recognized or they may trigger mental models for several diseases at once. When this happens, slow system 2 processing may be engaged, and the clinician will continue to gather, integrate, and interpret potentially relevant information until a working diagnosis is generated and communicated to the patient. When this process triggers pattern matches for several mental models of disease, a differential diagnosis is developed. At this point, the diagnostic process shifts to slow system 2 analytical reasoning. Based on their knowledge base, clinicians then use deductive reasoning: If this patient has disease A, what clinical history and physical examination findings might be expected, and does the patient have them? This process is repeated for each condition in the differential diagnosis and may be augmented by additional sources of information, such as diagnostic testing, further history gathering or physical examination, or referral or consultation. The cognitive process of reassessing the probability assigned to each potential diagnosis involves inductive reasoning,5 or going from observed signs and symptoms to the likelihood of each disease to determine which hypothesis is most likely (Goodman, 1999). This can help refine and narrow the differential diagnosis. Further information gathering activities or treatment could provide greater certainty regarding a working diagnosis or suggest that alternative diagnoses be considered. Throughout this process, clinicians need to communicate with patients about the working diagnosis and the degree of certainty involved.

Task complexity and expertise affect which cognitive system is dominantly employed in the diagnostic process. System 1 processing is more likely to be used when patients present with typical signs and symptoms of disease. However, system 2 processing is likely to intervene in situations marked by novelty and difficulty, when patients present with atypical signs and symptoms, or when clinicians lack expertise (Croskerry, 2009b; Evans and Stanovich, 2013). Novice clinicians and medical students are more likely to rely on analytical reasoning throughout the diagnostic process compared to experienced clinicians (Croskerry, 2009b; Elstein and Schwartz, 2002; Kassirer, 2010; Norman, 2005). Expert clinicians possess better developed mental models of diseases, which support more reliable pattern matching (system 1 processes) (Croskerry, 2009b). As a clinician accumulates experience, the repetition of system 2 processing can expand pattern matching possibilities by building and storing in memory mental models for additional diseases that can be triggered by patient signs and symptoms. The ability to create and develop mental models through repetition explains why expert clinicians are more likely to rely on pattern recognition when making diagnoses than are novices—continuous engagement with disease conditions allows the expert to develop more reliable mental models of disease—by retaining more exemplars, creating more nuanced prototypes, or developing more detailed illness scripts.

The way in which information is processed through system 1 and system 2 informs a clinician's subsequent diagnostic performance. Figure 2-3 illustrates the concept of calibration, or the process of a clinician becoming aware of his or her diagnostic abilities and limitations through feedback. Feedback mechanisms—both in educational settings (see Chapter 4) and in learning health care systems (see Chapter 6)—allow clinicians to compare their patients' ultimate diagnoses with the diagnoses that they provided to those patients. Calibration enables clinicians to assess their diagnostic accuracy and improve their future performance.

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

FIGURE 2-3

Calibration in the diagnostic process. Favorable or unfavorable information about a clinician's diagnostic performance provides good feedback and improves clinician calibration. When a patient's diagnostic outcome is unknown, it will be treated as favorable (more...)

Work system factors influence diagnostic reasoning, including diagnostic team members and tasks, technologies and tools, organizational characteristics, the physical environment, and the external environment. For example, Chapter 6 describes how the physical environment, including lighting, noise, and layout, can influence clinical reasoning. Chapter 5 discusses how health IT can improve or degrade clinical reasoning, depending on the usability of health IT (including clinical decision support), its integration into clinical workflow, and other factors. Box 2-5 describes how certain individual characteristics of diagnostic team members can affect clinical reasoning.

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

BOX 2-5

Individual Characteristics That Influence Clinical Reasoning.

Probabilistic (Bayesian) Reasoning

As described above, the diagnostic process involves initial information gathering that leads to a working diagnosis. The process of ruling in or ruling out a diagnosis involves probabilistic reasoning as findings are integrated and interpreted. Probabilistic (or Bayesian) reasoning provides a formal method to avoid some cognitive biases when integrating and interpreting information. For instance, when patients present with typical symptoms but the disease is rare (e.g., the classic triad of headache, sweating, and rapid heart rate for pheochromocytoma), base rate neglect and the representativeness bias may lead clinicians to overestimate the likelihood of pheochromocytoma among patients presenting with high blood pressure. Using Bayesian reasoning and formally revising probabilities of the various diseases under consideration helps clinicians avoid these errors. Clinicians can then decide whether to pursue additional information gathering or treatment based on an accurate estimate of the likelihood of disease, the harms and benefits of treatment, and patient preferences (Kassirer et al., 2010; Pauker and Kassirer, 1980).

Probabilistic reasoning is most often considered in the context of diagnostic testing, but the presence or absence of specific signs and symptoms can also help to rule in or rule out diseases. The likelihood of a positive finding (the presence of signs or symptoms or a positive test) when disease is present is referred to as sensitivity. The likelihood of a negative finding (the absence of symptoms, signs, or a negative test) when a disease is absent is referred to as specificity. If a sign, symptom, or test is always positive in the presence of a particular disease (100 percent sensitivity), then the absence of that symptom, sign, or test rules out disease (e.g., absence of pain or stiffness means the patient does not have polymyalgia rheumatica). If a sign, symptom, or test is always negative in the absence of a particular disease (100 percent specificity), then the presence of that symptom, sign, or test rules in disease (e.g., all patients with Kayser–Fleischer rings have Wilson's disease; all patients with Koplik's spots have measles).

However, nearly all signs, symptoms, or test results are neither 100 percent sensitive or specific. For example, studies suggest exceptions for findings such as Kayser–Fleischer rings with other causes of liver disease (Frommer et al., 1977; Lipman and Deutsch, 1990) or Koplik's spots with parvovirus B19 or echovirus (Suringa et al., 1970) and even for Reed-Sternburg cells for Hodgkin's lymphoma (Azar, 1975).

Bayes' theorem provides a framework for clinicians to revise the probability of disease, given disease prevalence, as well as the presence or absence of clinical findings or positive or negative test results (Grimes and Schulz, 2005; Griner et al., 1981; Kassirer et al., 2010; Pauker and Kassirer, 1980). Bayesian calculators are available to facilitate these probability revision analyses (Simel and Rennie, 2008). Box 2-6 works through two examples of probabilistic reasoning. While most clinicians will not formally calculate probabilities, the logical principles behind Bayesian reasoning can help clinicians consider the trade-offs involved in further information gathering, decisions about treatment, or evaluating clinically ambiguous cases (Kassirer et al., 2010). The committee's recommendation on improving diagnostic competencies includes a focus on diagnostic test ordering and subsequent decision making, which relies on the principles of probabilistic reasoning.

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

BOX 2-6

Examples of Probabilistic (Bayesian) Reasoning.

THE DIAGNOSTIC EVIDENCE BASE AND CLINICAL PRACTICE

Advances in biology and medicine have led to improvements in prevention, diagnosis, and treatment, with a deluge of innovations in diagnostic testing (IOM, 2000, 2013a; Korf and Rehm, 2013; Lee and Levy, 2012). The rising complexity and volume of these advances, coupled with clinician time constraints and cognitive limitations, have outstripped human capacity to apply this new knowledge (IOM, 2011a, 2013a; Marois and Ivanoff, 2005; Miller, 1956; Ostbye et al., 2005; Tombu et al., 2011; Yarnall et al., 2003). The Institute of Medicine report Best Care Lower Cost: The Path to Continuously Learning Health Care in America concluded that “diagnostic and treatment options are expanding and changing at an accelerating rate, placing new stresses on clinicians and patients, as well as potentially impacting the effectiveness and efficiency of care delivery” (IOM, 2013a, p. 10). The sheer number of potential diagnoses illustrates this complexity: There are thousands of diseases and related health conditions categorized in the National Library of Medicine's medical subjects headings system and around 13,000 in International Classification of Diseases, 9th Edition, with new conditions and diseases added every year (Medicaid.gov, 2015).

With the rapidly increasing number of published scientific articles on health (see Figure 2-4), health care professionals have difficulty keeping up with the breadth and depth of knowledge in their specialties. For example, to remain up to date, primary care clinicians would need to read for an estimated 627.5 hours per month (Alper et al., 2004). McGlynn and colleagues (2003) found that Americans receive only about half of recommended care, including recommended diagnostic processes. Thus, clinicians need approaches to ensure they know the evidence base and are well-equipped to deliver care that reflects the most up-to-date information. One of the ways that this is accomplished is through team-based care; by moving from individuals to teams of health care professionals, patients can benefit from a broader set of resources and expertise to support care (Gittell et al., 2010) (see Chapter 4). In addition, systematic reviews and clinical practice guidelines (CPGs) help synthesize available information in order to inform clinical practice decision making (IOM, 2011a,b).

The typical amount of time for a complete physical examination is __________.

FIGURE 2-4

Number of journal articles published on health care topics per year from 1970 to 2010. Publications have increased steadily over 40 years. SOURCE: IOM, 2013a.

CPGs came into prominence partly in response to studies that found excessive variation in diagnostic and treatment-related care practices, indicating that inappropriate care was occurring (Chassin et al., 1987; IOM, 1990; Kosecoff et al., 1987; Lin et al., 2008; Song et al., 2010). CPGs are defined as “statements that include recommendations intended to optimize patient care that are informed by a systematic review of the evidence and an assessment of the benefits and harms of alternative care options” (IOM, 2011a, p. 4). CPGs can include diagnostic criteria for specific conditions as well as approaches to information gathering, such as conducting a clinical history and interview, the physical exam, diagnostic testing, and consultations.

CPGs translate knowledge into clinical care decisions, and adherence to evidence-based guideline recommendations can improve health care quality and patient outcomes (Bhatt et al., 2004; IOM, 2011a; Peterson et al., 2006). However, there have been a number of challenges to the development and use of CPGs in clinical practice (IOM, 2011a, 2013a,b; Kahn et al., 2014; Timmermans and Mauck, 2005). Two of the primary challenges are the inadequacy of the evidence base supporting CPGs and determining the applicability of guidelines for individual patients (IOM, 2011a, 2013b). For example, individual patient preferences for possible health outcomes may vary, and with the growing prevalence of chronic disease, patients often have comorbidities or competing causes of mortality that need to be considered. CPGs may not factor in these patient-specific variables (Boyd et al., 2005; Mulley et al., 2012; Tinetti et al., 2004). In addition, the majority of scientific evidence about any diagnostic test typically is focused on test accuracy and not on the impact of the test on patient outcomes (Brozek et al., 2009; Trikalinos et al., 2009). This makes it difficult to develop guidelines that inform clinicians about the role of diagnostic tests within the diagnostic process and about how these tests can influence the path of care and health outcomes for a patient (Gopalakrishna et al., 2014; Hsu et al., 2011). Furthermore, diagnosis is generally not a primary focus of CPGs; diagnostic testing guidelines typically account for a minority of recommendations and often have lower levels of evidence supporting them than treatment-related CPGs (Tricoci et al., 2009). The adoption of available clinical practice guideline recommendations into practice remains suboptimal due to concerns about the trustworthiness of the guidelines as well as the existence of varying and conflicting guidelines (Ferket et al., 2011; Han et al., 2011; IOM, 2011a; Lenzer et al., 2013; Pronovost, 2013).

Health care professional societies have also begun to develop appropriate use or appropriateness criteria as a way of synthesizing the available scientific literature and expert opinion to inform patient-specific decision making (Fitch et al., 2001). With the growth of diagnostic testing and substantial geographic variation in the utilization of these tools (due in part to the limitations in the evidence base supporting their use), health care professional societies have developed appropriate use criteria aimed at better matching patients to specific health care interventions (Allen and Thorwarth, 2014; Patel et al., 2005).

Checklists are another approach that has been implemented to improve the safety of care by, for example, preventing health care–acquired infections or errors in surgical care. Checklists have also been proposed to improve the diagnostic process (Ely et al., 2011; Schiff and Leape, 2012; Sibbald et al., 2013). Developing checklists for the diagnostic process may be a significant undertaking; thus far, checklists have been developed for discrete, observable tasks, but the complexity of the diagnostic process, including the associated cognitive tasks, may represent a fundamentally different type of challenge (Henriksen and Brady, 2013).

REFERENCES

  • ACMG (American College of Medical Genetics and Genomics) Board of Directors. Points to consider in the clinical application in genomic sequencing. Genetics in Medicine. 2012;14(8):759–761. [PubMed: 22863877]

  • Allen B, Thorwarth WT. Comments from the American College of Radiology. Washington, DC: 2014. (Input submitted to the Committee on Diagnostic Error in Health Care, November 5 and December 29, 2014).

  • Alper B, Hand JA, Elliott SG, Kinkade S, Hauan MJ, Onion DK, Sklar BM. How much effort is needed to keep up with the literature relevant for primary care? Journal of the Medical Library Association. 2004;92(4):429–437. [PMC free article: PMC521514] [PubMed: 15494758]

  • Azar HA. Significance of the Reed-Sternberg cell. Human Pathology. 1975;6(4):479–484. [PubMed: 1150223]

  • Barrows HS. Problem-based learning: An approach to medical education. New York: Springer; 1980.

  • Barrows HS, Norman GR, Neufeld VR, Feightner JW. The clinical reasoning of randomly selected physicians in general medical practice. Clinical & Investigative Medicine. 1982;5(1):49–55. [PubMed: 7116714]

  • Bayer AJ, Chadha JS, Farag RR, Pathy MS. Changing presentation of myocardial infarction with increasing old age. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1986;34(4):263–266. [PubMed: 3950299]

  • Berger D. A brief history of medical diagnosis and the birth of the clinical laboratory. Part 4—Fraud and abuse, managed-care, and lab consolidation. Medical Laboratory Observer. 1999;31(12):38–42. [PubMed: 11184281]

  • Berlin L. Radiologic errors, past, present and future. Diagnosis. 2014;1(1):79–84. [PubMed: 29539959]

  • Berner ES, Graber ML. Overconfidence as a cause of diagnostic error in medicine. The American Journal of Medicine. 2008;121(5):S2–S23. [PubMed: 18440350]

  • Bhatt DL, Roe MT, Peterson ED, Li Y, Chen AY, Harrington RA, Greenbaum AB, Berger PB, Cannon CP, Cohen DJ, Gibson CM, Saucedo JF, Kleiman NS, Hochman JS, Boden WE, Brindis RG, Peacock WF, Smith SC Jr., Pollack CV Jr., Gibler WB, Ohman EM. CRUSADE Investigators. Utilization of early invasive management strategies for high-risk patients with non-ST-segment elevation acute coronary syndromes: Results from the CRUSADE Quality Improvement Initiative. JAMA. 2004;292(17):2096–2104. [PubMed: 15523070]

  • Blanchette I, Richards A. The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition and Emotion. 2009;24(4):561–595.

  • Bluth EI, Truong H, Bansal S. The 2014 ACR Commission on Human Resources Workforce Survey. Journal of the American College of Radiology. 2014;11(10):948–952. [PubMed: 25131824]

  • Bor JS. Among the elderly, many mental illnesses go undiagnosed. Health Affairs (Millwood). 2015;34(5):727–731. [PubMed: 25941272]

  • Bordage G, Zacks R. The structure of medical knowledge in the memories of medical students and general practitioners: categories and prototypes. Medical Education. 1984;18(6):406–416. [PubMed: 6503748]

  • Boshuizen HPA, Schmidt HG. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Higgs J, Jones M, Loftus S, Christensen N, editors. Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann/Elsevier; 2008. pp. 113–121. (The development of clinical reasoning expertise; Implications for teaching).

  • Boyd CM, Darer J, Boult C, Fried LP, Boult L, Wu AW. Clinical practice guidelines and quality of care for older patients with multiple comorbid diseases: Implications for pay for performance. JAMA. 2005;294(6):716–724. [PubMed: 16091574]

  • Brozek JL, Akl EA, Jaeschke R, Lang DM, Bossuyt P, Glasziou P, Helfand M, Ueffing E, Alonso-Coello P, Meerpohl J, Phillips B, Horvath AR, Bousquet J, Guyatt GH, Schunemann HJ, Group GW. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations in clinical practice guidelines: Part 2 of 3. The GRADE approach to grading quality of evidence about diagnostic tests and strategies. Allergy. 2009;64(8):1109–1116. [PubMed: 19489757]

  • Byrnes JP, Miller DC, Schafer WD. Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125(3):367.

  • Campbell VA, Crews JE, Moriarty DG, Zack MM, Blackman DK. Surveillance for sensory impairment, activity limitation, and health-related quality of life among older adults—United States, 1993-1997. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 1999;48(SS08):131–156. [PubMed: 10634273]

  • Carayon P, Schoofs Hundt A, Karsh BT, Gurses AP, Alvarado CJ, Smith M, Flatley Brennan P. Work system design for patient safety: The SEIPS model. Quality & Safety in Health Care. 2006;15(Suppl 1):i50–i58. [PMC free article: PMC2464868] [PubMed: 17142610]

  • Carayon P, Wetterneck TB, Rivera-Rodriguez AJ, Hundt AS, Hoonakker P, Holden R, Gurses AP. Human factors systems approach to healthcare quality and patient safety. Applied Ergonomics. 2014;45(1):14–25. [PMC free article: PMC3795965] [PubMed: 23845724]

  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). Laboratory medicine: A national status report. Church, VA: The Lewin Group; Falls. 2008.

  • CDC. National hospital ambulatory medical care survey. Hyattsville, MD: Ambulatory and Hospital Care Statistics Branch, National Center for Health Statistics; 2010.

  • CDC. Clinical Laboratory Improvement Amendments (CLIA). 2014. [May 15, 2015]. www​.cdc.gov/clia.

  • Centor RM, Witherspoon JM, Dalton HP, Brody CE, Link K. The diagnosis of strep throat in adults in the emergency room. Medical decision making: an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making. 1980;1(3):239–246. [PubMed: 6763125]

  • Chassin MR, Kosecoff J, Solomon DH, Brook RH. How coronary angiography is used: Clinical determinants of appropriateness. JAMA. 1987;258(18):2543–2547. [PubMed: 3312657]

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J. Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition. 1996;58(1):1–73.

  • Croskerry P. The feedback sanction. Academic Emergency Medicine. 2000;7(11):1232–1238. [PubMed: 11073471]

  • Croskerry P. The Importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to minimize them. Academic Medicine. 2003a;78(8):775–780. [PubMed: 12915363]

  • Croskerry P. Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical decisionmaking. Annals of Emergency Medicine. 2003b;41(1):110–120. [PubMed: 12514691]

  • Croskerry P. Clinical cognition and diagnostic error: Applications of a dual process model of reasoning. Advances in Health Sciences Education. 2009a;14(Suppl 1):27–35. [PubMed: 19669918]

  • Croskerry P. A universal model of diagnostic reasoning. Academic Medicine. 2009b;84(8):1022–1028. [PubMed: 19638766]

  • Croskerry P, Musson D. Patient Safety in Emergency Medicine. Croskerry P, Cosby KS, Schenkel SM, Wears RL, editors. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2009. pp. 269–276. (Individual factors in patient safety).

  • Croskerry P, Norman G. Overconfidence in clinical decision making. American Journal of Medicine. 2008;121(5 Suppl):S24–S29. [PubMed: 18440351]

  • Croskerry P, Abbass AA, Wu AW. How doctors feel: affective issues in patients' safety. Lancet. 2008;372(9645):1205–1206. [PubMed: 19094942]

  • Croskerry P, Abbass AA, Wu AW. Emotional influences in patient safety. Journal of Patient Safety. 2010;6(4):199–205. [PubMed: 21500605]

  • Croskerry P, Singhal G, Mamede S. Cognitive debiasing 1: Origins of bias and theory of debiasing. BMJ Quality and Safety. 2013;22(Suppl 2):ii58–ii64. [PMC free article: PMC3786658] [PubMed: 23882089]

  • Davies RH, Rees B. Include “eyeballing” the patient. BMJ. 2010;340(c291) [PubMed: 20085978]

  • Eichbaum Q, Booth GS, Young PS, editors; Laposata M, editor. Transfusion medicine: Quality in laboratory diagnosis. New York: Demos Medical Publishing; 2012.

  • Elstein AS, Bordage G. Professional judgment: A reader in clinical decision making. Dowie J, Elstein A, editors. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1988. pp. 109–129. (Psychology of clinical reasoning).

  • Elstein AS, Schwartz A. Clinical problem solving and diagnostic decision making: Selective review of the cognitive literature. BMJ. 2002;324(7339):729–732. [PMC free article: PMC1122649] [PubMed: 11909793]

  • Elstein AS, Shulman L, Sprafka S. Medical problem solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978.

  • Elstein AS, Shulman LS, Sprafka SA. Medical problem solving: A ten-year retrospective. Evaluation & the Health Professions. 1990;13(1):5–36.

  • Ely JW, Graber ML, Croskerry P. Checklists to reduce diagnostic errors. Academic Medicine. 2011;86(3):307–313. [PubMed: 21248608]

  • Emmett KR. Nonspecific and atypical presentation of disease in the older patient. Geriatrics. 1998;53(2):50–52. 58-60. [PubMed: 9484285]

  • Epner PL, Gans JE, Graber ML. When diagnostic testing leads to harm: A new outcomes-based approach for laboratory medicine. BMJ Quality and Safety. 2013;22(Suppl 2):ii6–ii10. [PMC free article: PMC3786651] [PubMed: 23955467]

  • Eva KW. The aging physician: Changes in cognitive processing and their impact on medical practice. Academic Medicine. 2002;77(10 Suppl):S1–S6. [PubMed: 12377689]

  • Eva KW, Cunnington JPW. The difficulty with experience: Does practice increase susceptibility to premature closure? Journal of Continuing Education in the Health Professions. 2006;26(3):192–198. [PubMed: 16986144]

  • Eva K, Link C, Lutfey K, McKinlay J. Swapping horses midstream: Factors related to physicians changing their minds about a diagnosis. Academic Medicine. 2010;85:1112–1117. [PMC free article: PMC3701113] [PubMed: 20592506]

  • Evans JP, Watson MS. Genetic testing and FDA regulation: Overregulation threatens the emergence of genomic medicine. JAMA. 2015;313(7):669–670. [PubMed: 25560537]

  • Evans JSBT, Stanovich KE. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2013;8(3):223–241. [PubMed: 26172965]

  • Ferket BS, Genders TS, Colkesen EB, Visser JJ, Spronk S, Steyerberg EW, Hunink MG. Systematic review of guidelines on imaging of asymptomatic coronary artery disease. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2011;57(15):1591–1600. [PubMed: 21474039]

  • Fitch K, Bernstein SJ, Aguilar MD, Burnand B, LaCalle JR, Lazaro P, Loo Mvh, McDonnell J, Vader J, Kahan JP. The RAND/UCLA appropriateness method user 's manual. 2001. [May 13, 2015]. www​.rand.org/pubs/monograph_reports​/MR1269.

  • Flores G. Language barriers to health care in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine. 2006;355(3):229–231. [PubMed: 16855260]

  • Frommer D, Morris J, Sherlock S, Abrams J, Newman S. Kayser-Fleischer-like rings in patients without Wilson's disease. Gastroenterology. 1977;72(6):1331–1335. [PubMed: 558126]

  • Gandhi JS. Re: William Osler: A life in medicine: Book review. BMJ. 2000;321:1087.

  • Gigerenzer G. Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000.

  • Gigerenzer G. HIV screening: Helping clinicians make sense of test results to patients. BMJ. 2013;347:f5151. [PubMed: 23965510]

  • Gigerenzer G, Goldstein DG. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychology Review. 1996;103:650–669. [PubMed: 8888650]

  • Gittell JH, Seidner R, Wimbush J. A relational model of how high-performance work systems work. Organization Science. 2010;21(2):490–506.

  • Goodman SN. Toward evidence-based medical statistics. 1: The P value fallacy. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1999;130(12):995–1004. [PubMed: 10383371]

  • Gopalakrishna G, Mustafa RA, Davenport C, Scholten RJPM, Hyde C, Brozek J, Schunemann HJ, Bossuyt PMM, Leeflang MMG, Langendam MW. Applying Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) to diagnostic tests was challenging but doable. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2014;67(7):760–768. [PubMed: 24725643]

  • Graber ML. Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2005;165(13):1493–1499. [PubMed: 16009864]

  • Gray-Miceli D. Modification of assessment and atypical presentation in older adults with complex illness. New York: The John A. Hartford Foundation Institute for Geriatric Nursing; 2008.

  • Grimes DA, Schulz KF. Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios. Lancet. 2005;365(9469):1500–1505. [PubMed: 15850636]

  • Griner PF, Mayewski RJ, Mushlin AI, Greenland P. Selection and interpretation of diagnostic tests and procedures: Principles and applications. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1981;94(4 Pt 2):557–592. [PubMed: 6452080]

  • Groen GJ, Patel VL. Medical problem-solving: Some questionable assumptions. Medical Education. 1985;19(2):95–100. [PubMed: 3982318]

  • Gunderman RB. The medical community's changing vision of the patient: The importance of radiology. Radiology. 2005;234(2):339–342. [PubMed: 15670989]

  • Han PK, Klabunde CN, Breen N, Yuan G, Grauman A, Davis WW, Taplin SH. Multiple clinical practice guidelines for breast and cervical cancer screening: perceptions of U.S. primary care physicians. Medical Care. 2011;49(2):139–148. [PMC free article: PMC4207297] [PubMed: 21206294]

  • Hendee WR, Becker GJ, Borgstede JP, Bosma J, Casarella WJ, Erickson BA, Maynard CD, Thrall JH, Wallner PE. Addressing overutilization in medical imaging. Radiology. 2010;257(1):240–245. [PubMed: 20736333]

  • Henriksen K, Brady J. The pursuit of better diagnostic performance: A human factors perspective. BMJ Quality and Safety. 2013;22(Suppl 2):ii1–ii5. [PMC free article: PMC3786636] [PubMed: 23704082]

  • Hickner J, Thompson PJ, Wilkinson T, Epner P, Shaheen M, Pollock AM, Lee J, Duke CC, Jackson BR, Taylor JR. Primary care physicians' challenges in ordering clinical laboratory tests and interpreting results. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine. 2014;27(2):268–274. [PubMed: 24610189]

  • Hoffman KA, Aitken LM, Duffield C. A comparison of novice and expert nurses' cue collection during clinical decision-making: Verbal protocol analysis. International Journal of Nursing Studies. 2009;46(10):1335–1344. [PubMed: 19555954]

  • Hollensead SC, Lockwood WB, Elin RJ. Errors in pathology and laboratory medicine: Consequences and prevention. Journal of Surgical Oncology. 2004;88(3):161–181. [PubMed: 15562462]

  • Holmboe ES, Durning SJ. Assessing clinical reasoning: Moving from in vitro to in vivo. Diagnosis. 2014;1(1):111–117. [PubMed: 29539977]

  • Hope C, Estrada N, Weir C, Teng CC, Damal K, Sauer BC. Documentation of delirium in the VA electronic health record. BMC Research Notes. 2014;7:208. [PMC free article: PMC3985575] [PubMed: 24708799]

  • Hricak H. Oncologic imaging: A guiding hand of personalized cancer care. Radiology. 2011;259(3):633–640. [PubMed: 21493796]

  • Hsu J, Brozek JL, Terracciano L, Kreis J, Compalati E, Stein AT, Fiocchi A, Schunemann HJ. Application of GRADE: Making evidence-based recommendations about diagnostic tests in clinical practice guidelines. Implementation Science. 2011;6:62. [PMC free article: PMC3126717] [PubMed: 21663655]

  • IOM (Institute of Medicine). Clinical practice guidelines: Directions for a new program. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1990. [PubMed: 25144032]

  • IOM. Medicare laboratory payment policy: Now and in the future. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000. [PubMed: 25057735]

  • IOM. Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic disparties in health care. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2003. [PubMed: 25032386]

  • IOM. Improving breast imaging quality standards. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2005.

  • IOM. Cancer biomarkers: The promises and challenges of improving detection and treatment. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2007.

  • IOM. Retooling for an aging America: Building the health care workforce. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2008. [PubMed: 25009893]

  • IOM. Evaluation of biomarkers and surrogate endpoints in chronic disease. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2010. [PubMed: 25032382]

  • IOM. Clinical practice guidelines we can trust. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011a. [PubMed: 24983061]

  • IOM. Finding what works in health care: Standards for systematic reviews. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011b. [PubMed: 24983062]

  • IOM. Evolution of translational omics: Lessons learned and the path forward. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2012. [PubMed: 24872966]

  • IOM. Best care at lower cost: The path to continuously learning health care in America. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2013a. [PubMed: 24901184]

  • IOM. Delivering high-quality cancer care: Charting a new course for a system in crisis. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2013b. [PubMed: 24872984]

  • IOM. Improving genetics education in graduate and continuing health professional education: Workshop summary. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2015a. [PubMed: 25674655]

  • Jameson JL, Longo DL. Precision medicine—Personalized, problematic, and promising. New England Journal of Medicine. 2015;372(23):2229–2234. [PubMed: 26014593]

  • Jarrett PG, Rockwood K, Carver D, Stolee P, Cosway S. Illness presentation in elderly patients. Archives of Internal Medicine. 1995;155(10):1060–1064. [PubMed: 7748049]

  • Johansen Taber KA, Dickinson BD, Wilson M. The promise and challenges of next-generation genome sequencing for clinical care. JAMA Internal Medicine. 2014;174(2):275–280. [PubMed: 24217348]

  • Johnson-Laird PN, Oatley K. Basic emotions, rationality, and folk theory. Cognition & Emotion. 1992;6(3-4):201–223.

  • Jutel A. Sociology of diagnosis: A preliminary review. Sociology of Health and Illness. 2009;31(2):278–299. [PubMed: 19220801]

  • Kahn JM, Gould MK, Krishnan JA, Wilson KC, Au DH, Cooke CR, Douglas IS, Feemster LC, Mularski RA, Slatore CG, Wiener RS. An official American thoracic society workshop report: Developing performance measures from clinical practice guidelines. Annals of the American Thoracic Society. 2014;11(4):S186–S195. [PMC free article: PMC5469393] [PubMed: 24828810]

  • Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux; 2011.

  • Kahneman D, Klein G. Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. American Psychologist. 2009;64(6):515–526. [PubMed: 19739881]

  • Kanwisher N, Yovel G. The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2006;361(1476):2109–2128. [PMC free article: PMC1857737] [PubMed: 17118927]

  • Kassirer JP. Our stubborn quest for diagnostic certainty. A cause of excessive testing. New England Journal of Medicine. 1989;320(22):1489–1491. [PubMed: 2497349]

  • Kassirer JP. Teaching clinical reasoning: Case-based and coached. Academic Medicine. 2010;85(7):1118–1124. [PubMed: 20603909]

  • Kassirer JP. Imperatives, expediency, and the new diagnosis. Diagnosis. 2014;1(1):11–12. [PubMed: 29539968]

  • Kassirer JP, Wong J, Kopelman R. Learning clinical reasoning. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 2010.

  • Kent DM, Hayward RA. Limitations of applying summary results of clinical trials to individual patients: The need for risk stratification. JAMA. 2007;298(10):1209–1212. [PubMed: 17848656]

  • Klein G. Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998.

  • Kleinman S, Busch MP, Hall L, Thomson R, Glynn S, Gallahan D, Ownby HE, Williams AE. False-positive HIV-1 test results in a low-risk screening setting of voluntary blood donation: Retrovirus Epidemiology Donor Study. JAMA. 1998;280(12):1080–1085. [PubMed: 9757856]

  • Korf BR, Rehm HL. New approaches to molecular diagnosis. JAMA. 2013;309(14):1511–1521. [PubMed: 23571590]

  • Kosecoff J, Chassin MR, Fink A, Flynn MF, McCloskey L, Genovese BJ, Oken C, Solomon DH, Brook RH. Obtaining clinical data on the appropriateness of medical care in community practice. JAMA. 1987;258(18):2538–2542. [PubMed: 3312656]

  • Kostopoulou O, Rosen A, Round T, Wright E, Douiri A, Delaney B. Early diagnostic suggestions improve accuracy of GPs: A randomised controlled trial using computer-simulated patients. British Journal of General Practice. 2015;65(630):e49–e54. [PMC free article: PMC4276007] [PubMed: 25548316]

  • Krupinski EA, Berbaum KS, Caldwell RT, Schartz KM, Madsen MT, Kramer DJ. Do long radiology workdays affect nodule detection in dynamic CT interpretation? Journal of the American College of Radiology. 2012;9(3):191–198. [PMC free article: PMC3296477] [PubMed: 22386166]

  • Laposata M. Coagulation disorders: Quality in laboratory diagnosis. New York: Demos Medical Publishing; 2010.

  • Laposata M, Dighe A. “Pre-pre” and “post-post” analytical error: High-incidence patient safety hazards involving the clinical laboratory. Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine. 2007;45(6):712–719. [PubMed: 17579522]

  • Lee DW, Levy F. The sharp slowdown in growth of medical imaging: an early analysis suggests combination of policies was the cause. Health Affairs (Millwood). 2012;31(8):1876–1884. [PubMed: 22842655]

  • Lenzer J, Hoffman JR, Furberg CD, Ioannidis JP. Ensuring the integrity of clinical practice guidelines: A tool for protecting patients. BMJ. 2013;347:f5535. on behalf of the Guideline Panel Review Working Group. [PubMed: 24046286]

  • Lichstein PR. Clinical Methods: The History, Physical, and Laboratory Examinations, 3rd edition. Walker HK, Hall WD, Hurst JW, editors. Boston: Butterworths; 1990. (The medical interview). [PubMed: 21250045]

  • Lin GA, Dudley RA, Lucas FL, Malenka DJ, Vittinghoff E, Redberg RF. Frequency of stress testing to document ischemia prior to elective percutaneous coronary intervention. JAMA. 2008;300(15):1765–1773. [PubMed: 18854538]

  • Lipman RM, Deutsch TA. A yellow-green posterior limbal ring in a patient who does not have Wilson's disease. Archives of Ophthalmology. 1990;108(10):1385. [PubMed: 2222268]

  • Lipshitz R, Klein G, Orasanu J, Salas E. Taking stock of naturalistic decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2001;14(5):331–352.

  • Loewenstein G, Lerner JS. Handbook of affective sciences. Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 619–642. (The role of affect in decision making).

  • Lundberg GD. Acting on significant laboratory results. JAMA. 1981;245(17):1762–1763. [PubMed: 7218491]

  • Marois R, Ivanoff J. Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(6):296–305. [PubMed: 15925809]

  • McDonald CJ. Medical heuristics: the silent adjudicators of clinical practice. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1996;124(1 Pt 1):56–62. [PubMed: 7503478]

  • McGlynn EA, Asch SM, Adams J, Keesey J, Hicks J, DeCristofaro A, Kerr EA. The quality of health care delivered to adults in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;348(26):2635–2645. [PubMed: 12826639]

  • McSherry D. Avoiding premature closure in sequential diagnosis. Artificial Intelligence in Medicine. 1997;10(3):269–283. [PubMed: 9232189]

  • Meyer F, Meyer TD. The misdiagnosis of bipolar disorder as a psychotic disorder: Some of its causes and their influence on therapy. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2009;112(1-3):174–183. [PubMed: 18555536]

  • Miller GA. The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review. 1956;63(2):81–97. [PubMed: 13310704]

  • Moroff SV, Pauker SG. What to do when the patient outlives the literature. Medical Decision Making. 1983;3(3):313–338. [PubMed: 6645821]

  • Mouton CP, Bazaldua OV, Pierce B, Espino DV. Common infections in older adults. American Family Physician. 2001;63(2):257–268. [PubMed: 11201692]

  • Mulley AG, Trimble C, Elwyn G. Stop the silent misdiagnosis: Patients' preferences matter. BMJ. 2012;345:e6572. [PubMed: 23137819]

  • Nelson C, Hojvat S, Johnson B, Petersen J, Schriefer M, Beard CB, Petersen L, Mead P. Concerns regarding a new culture method for Borrelia burgdorferi not approved for the diagnosis of Lyme disease. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2014;63(15):333. [PMC free article: PMC5779394] [PubMed: 24739342]

  • Neufeld V, Norman G, Feightner J, Barrows H. Clinical problem-solving by medical students: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis. Medical Education. 1981;15(5):315–322. [PubMed: 6973686]

  • NIA (National Institute on Aging). A clinician's handbook: Talking with your older patient. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health; 2008.

  • Nichols JH, Rauch CA, editors. Clinical chemistry. New York: Demos Medical Publishing; 2013.

  • NIH (National Institutes of Health). Precision Medicine Initiative. 2015. [May 22, 2015]. www​.nih.gov/precisionmedicine.

  • Norman GR. Research in clinical reasoning: Past history and current trends. Medical Education. 2005;39(4):418–427. [PubMed: 15813765]

  • Norman GR, Eva KW. Diagnostic error and clinical reasoning. Medical Education. 2010;44(1):94–100. [PubMed: 20078760]

  • Ostbye T, Yarnall KS, Krause KM, Pollak KI, Gradison M, Michener JL. Is there time for management of patients with chronic diseases in primary care? Annals of Family Medicine. 2005;3(3):209–214. [PMC free article: PMC1466884] [PubMed: 15928223]

  • Papp KK, Huang GC, Lauzon Clabo LM, Delva D, Fischer M, Konopasek L, Schwartzstein RM, Gusic M. Milestones of critical thinking: A developmental model for medicine and nursing. Academic Medicine. 2014;89(5):715–720. [PubMed: 24667504]

  • Parasuraman R, Sheridan TB, Wickens CD. A model for types and levels of human interaction with automation. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics—Part A: Systems and Humans. 2000;30(3):286–297. [PubMed: 11760769]

  • Patel MR, Spertus JA, Brindis RG, Hendel RC, Douglas PS, Peterson ED, Wolk MJ, Allen JM, Raskin IE. ACCF proposed method for evaluating the appropriateness of cardiovascular imaging. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2005;46(8):1606–1613. [PubMed: 16226195]

  • Pauker SG, Kassirer JP. Therapeutic decision making: A cost-benefit analysis. New England Journal of Medicine. 1975;293(5):229–234. [PubMed: 1143303]

  • Pauker SG, Kassirer JP. The threshold approach to clinical decision making. New England Journal of Medicine. 1980;302(20):1109–1117. [PubMed: 7366635]

  • Pelaccia T, Tardif J, Triby E, Charlin B. An analysis of clinical reasoning through a recent and comprehensive approach: The dual-process theory. Medical Education Online. 2011 March;14:16. [PMC free article: PMC3060310] [PubMed: 21430797]

  • Peterson ED, Roe MT, Mulgund J, DeLong ER, Lytle BL, Brindis RG, Smith SC Jr., Pollack CV Jr., Newby LK, Harrington RA, Gibler WB, Ohman EM. Association between hospital process performance and outcomes among patients with acute coronary syndromes. JAMA. 2006;295(16):1912–1920. [PubMed: 16639050]

  • Pincus H. Diagnostic error: Issues in behavioral health. Washington, DC: 2014. (Presentation to the Committee on Diagnostic Error in Health Care, November 6, 2014).

  • Plebani M, Lippi G. Closing the brain-to-brain loop in laboratory testing. Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicince. 2011;49(7):1131–1133. [PubMed: 21663564]

  • Plebani M, Laposata M, Lundberg GD. The brain-to-brain loop concept for laboratory testing 40 years after its introduction. American Journal of Clinical Pathology. 2011;136(6):829–833. [PubMed: 22095366]

  • Pope JH, Aufderheide TP, Ruthazer R, Woolard RH, Feldman JA, Beshansky JR, Griffith JL, Selker HP. Missed diagnoses of acute cardiac ischemia in the emergency department. New England Journal of Medicine. 2000;342(16):1163–1170. [PubMed: 10770981]

  • Porter ME. What is value in health care? New England Journal of Medicine. 2010;363(26):2477–2481. [PubMed: 21142528]

  • Pronovost PJ. Enhancing physicians' use of clinical guidelines. JAMA. 2013;310(23):2501–2502. [PubMed: 24310916]

  • Qureshi AM, McDonald L, Primrose WR. Management of myocardial infarction in the very elderly—Impact of clinical effectiveness on practice. Scottish Medical Journal. 2000;45(6):180–182. [PubMed: 11216310]

  • Reeves RR, Parker JD, Burke RS, Hart RH. Inappropriate psychiatric admission of elderly patients with unrecognized delirium. Southern Medical Journal. 2010;103(2):111–115. [PubMed: 20065900]

  • Rich MW. Epidemiology, clinical features, and prognosis of acute myocardial infarction in the elderly. American Journal of Geriatric Cardiology. 2006;15(1):7–11. quiz 12. [PubMed: 16415640]

  • Rosch E, Mervis CB. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology. 1975;7(4):573–605.

  • Sarter N. Use(r)-centered design of health IT: Challenges and lessons learned. Washington, DC: 2014. (Presentation to the Committee on Diagnostic Error in Health Care, August, 7, 2014).

  • Schiff GD, Leape LL. Commentary: How can we make diagnosis safer? Academic Medicine. 2012;87(2):135–138. [PubMed: 22273611]

  • Schmidt HG, Norman GR, Boshuizen HPA. A cognitive perspective on medical expertise: Theory and implications. Academic Medicine. 1990;65:611–621. [PubMed: 2261032]

  • Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, Kerner JF, Sistrunk S, Gersh BJ, Dube R, Taleghani CK, Burke JE, Williams S, Eisenberg JM, Escarce JJ. The effect of race and sex on physicians' recommendations for cardiac catheterization. New England Journal of Medicine. 1999;340(8):618–626. [PubMed: 10029647]

  • Schwartz LH, Panicek DM, Berk AR, Li Y, Hricak H. Improving communication of diagnostic radiology findings through structured reporting. Radiology. 2011;260(1):174–181. [PMC free article: PMC3121011] [PubMed: 21518775]

  • Seshia SS, Makhinson M, Phillips DF, Young GB. Evidence-informed person-centered healthcare (part I): Do “cognitive biases plus” at organizational levels influence quality of evidence? Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice. 2014a;20(6):734–747. [PubMed: 25429739]

  • Seshia SS, Makhinson M, Young GB. Evidence-informed person-centered health care (part II): Are “cognitive biases plus” underlying the EBM paradigm responsible for undermining the quality of evidence? Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice. 2014b;20(6):748–758. [PubMed: 25494630]

  • Sharfstein J. FDA regulation of laboratory-developed diagnostic tests: Protect the public, advance the science. JAMA. 2015;313(7):667–668. [PubMed: 25560381]

  • Sibbald M, de Bruin AB, van Merrienboer JJ. Checklists improve experts' diagnostic decisions. Medical Education. 2013;47(3):301–308. [PubMed: 23398016]

  • Simel D, Rennie D. The rational clinical examination: Evidence-based clinical diagnosis. McGraw-Hill Professional; 2008.

  • Singer T, Verhaeghen P, Ghisletta P, Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. The fate of cognition in very old age: Six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and Aging. 2003;18(2):318–331. [PubMed: 12825779]

  • Slovic P, Peters E. Risk perception and affect. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15(6):322–325.

  • Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. Journal of SocioEconomics. 2002;31(4):329–342.

  • Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis. 2004;24(2):311–322. [PubMed: 15078302]

  • Small SA. Age-related memory decline: Current concepts and future directions. Archives of Neurology. 2001;58(3):360–364. [PubMed: 11255438]

  • Smith EE, Medin DL. Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1981.

  • Smith EE, Medin DL. Foundations of cognitive psychology: Core readings. Levitin DJ, editor. Cambridge, MA: Bradford; 2002. pp. 277–292. (The exemplar view).

  • Smith MB, Sainfort PC. A balance theory of job design for stress reduction. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics. 1989;4(1):67–79.

  • Snow V, Mottur-Pilson C, Cooper RJ, Hoffman JR. Principles of appropriate antibiotic use for acute pharyngitis in adults. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2001;134(6):506–508. [PubMed: 11255529]

  • Song Y, Skinner J, Bynum J, Sutherland J, Wennberg JE, Fisher ES. Regional variations in diagnostic practices. New England Journal of Medicine. 2010;363(1):45–53. [PMC free article: PMC2924574] [PubMed: 20463332]

  • Sox HC Jr. Probability theory in the use of diagnostic tests. An introduction to critical study of the literature. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1986;104(1):60–66. [PubMed: 3079637]

  • Stanovich KE. Decision making and rationality in the modern world. New York: Oxford; 2009.

  • Stanovich KE, Toplak ME. Defining features versus incidental correlates of Type 1 and Type 2 processing. Mind & Society. 2012;11(1):3–13.

  • Stark M, Fins JJ. The ethical imperative to think about thinking—Diagnostics, metacognition, and medical professionalism. Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics. 2014;23(4):386–396. [PubMed: 25033249]

  • Stratton CW. Clinical microbiology: Quality in laboratory diagnosis. New York: Demos Medical Publishing; 2011.

  • Suringa DWR, Bank LJ, Ackerman AB. Role of measles virus in skin lesions and Koplik's spots. New England Journal of Medicine. 1970;283(21):1139–1142. [PubMed: 5474350]

  • Timbie JW, Hussey PS, Burgette LF, Wenger NS, Rastegar A, Brantely I, Khodyakov D, Leuschner KJ, Weidmer BA, Kahn KL. Medicare imaging demonstration final evaluation: Report to Congress. Santa Monica, CA: RAND; 2014. [PMC free article: PMC5158237] [PubMed: 28083357]

  • Timmermans S, Mauck A. The promises and pitfalls of evidence-based medicine. Health Affairs (Millwood). 2005;24(1):18–28. [PubMed: 15647212]

  • Tinetti ME, Bogardus ST Jr., Agostini JV. Potential pitfalls of disease-specific guidelines for patients with multiple conditions. New England Journal of Medicine. 2004;351(27):2870–2874. [PubMed: 15625341]

  • Tombu MN, Asplund CL, Dux PE, Godwin D, Martin JW, Marois R. A unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academies of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(33):13426–13431. [PMC free article: PMC3158154] [PubMed: 21825137]

  • Tricoci P, Allen JM, Kramer JM, Califf RM, Smith SC Jr. Scientific evidence underlying the ACC/AHA clinical practice guidelines. JAMA. 2009;301(8):831–841. [Erratum appears in JAMA, 2009, 301(15):1544] [PubMed: 19244190]

  • Trikalinos TA, Siebert U, Lau J. Decision-analytic modeling to evaluate benefits and harms of medical tests: Uses and limitations. Medical Decision Making. 2009;29(5):E22–E29. [PubMed: 19734441]

  • Verghese A, Horwitz RI. In praise of the physical examination. BMJ. 2009;339:b5448. [PubMed: 20015910]

  • Verghese A, Brady E, Kapur CC, Horwitz RI. The bedside evaluation: ritual and reason. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2011;155(8):550–553. [PubMed: 22007047]

  • Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, Loewenstein G. Do emotions help or hurt decision making? A hedgefoxian perspective. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2007.

  • Welch HG. Less medicine more health: 7 assumptions that drive too much medical care. Boston, MA: Beacon Press; 2015.

  • WHO (World Health Organization). International classification of diseases (ICD). Geneva: World Health Organization; 2012.

  • Whiting P, Rutjes AWS, Reltsma JB, Glas AS, Bossuyt PMM, Kleljnen J. Sources of variation and bias in studies of diagnostic accuracy. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2004;140(3):189–202. [PubMed: 14757617]

  • Yarnall KS, Pollak KI, Ostbye T, Krause KM, Michener JL. Primary care: Is there enough time for prevention? American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(4):635–641. [PMC free article: PMC1447803] [PubMed: 12660210]

  • Zhi M, Ding EL, Theisen-Toupal J, Whelan J, Arnaout R. The landscape of inappropriate laboratory testing: A 15-year meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(11):e78962. [PMC free article: PMC3829815] [PubMed: 24260139]

  • Zwaan L, Thijs A, Wagner C, van der Wal G, Timmermans DR. Design of a study on suboptimal cognitive acts in the diagnostic process, the effect on patient outcomes and the influence of workload, fatigue and experience of physician. BMC Health Services Research. 2009;9:65. [PMC free article: PMC2680398] [PubMed: 19383168]

1

In this report, the committee employs the terminology “the diagnostic process” to convey diagnosis as a process.

2

The committee uses the term “diagnostic testing” to be inclusive of all types of testing, including medical imaging, anatomic pathology, and laboratory medicine, as well as other types of testing, such as mental health assessments, vision and hearing testing, and neurocognitive testing.

3

Public Law 110-275 (July 15, 2008).

4

The term “system 1” is an oversimplification because it is unlikely there is a single cognitive or neural system responsible for all system 1 cognitive processes.

5

Inductive reasoning involves probabilistic reasoning (see the following section).

What is the typical amount of time for a complete physical examination?

A thorough physical examination covers head to toe and usually lasts about 30 minutes. It measures important vital signs -- temperature, blood pressure, and heart rate -- and evaluates your body using observation, palpitation, percussion, and auscultation.

What is the maximum amount of time a patient should wait to be seen?

You should be aiming for the fewer-than-10-minute mark, as far as wait in the waiting room, and then less than 20 minutes from the time the patient is placed in the exam room until they see the doctor/practitioner (not the nurse/tech).

How much time is typically allotted for a routine Pap smear and pelvic examination?

It takes about 10-20 minutes for the whole exam, but only a few minutes for the actual Pap smear. The test is done in your doctor's office or clinic.

How much time is usually needed for an in office minor surgery such as a mole removal?

How long will removing a mole take? Typically you are in the office for about 30 minutes from start to finish.